1 / 50

Collectively, these recommendations include the following:

Collectively, these recommendations include the following:. 1. Embed learning in complex, realistic, and relevant environments. See, for example, Duffy and Cunningham (1996), CTGV (1991a 1992); Hannafin (1992), Honebein, Duffy, and Fishman (1993); and Lebow and Wager (1994).

casta
Télécharger la présentation

Collectively, these recommendations include the following:

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Collectively, these recommendations include the following:

  2. 1. Embed learning in complex, realistic, and relevant environments. See, for example, Duffy and Cunningham (1996), CTGV (1991a 1992); Hannafin (1992), Honebein, Duffy, and Fishman (1993); and Lebow and Wager (1994).

  3. This condition also find support in schema theory and mental models research (Chapter 4) as well as situated cognition (Chapter 5).

  4. 2. Provide for social negotiation as an integral part of learning. This learning condition is inherent in Piaget's theory (Chapter 6), Vygotsky's and Bruner's theories (Chapter 7), and situated cognition theory (Chapter 5).

  5. It also derives from the work of Cunningham (1992; Duffy & Cunningham, 1996), Honebein (1996; Honebein et al., 1993), CTGV (1990), and the Language Development and Hypermedia Group (1992a, 1992b), among others.

  6. 3. Support multiple perspectives and the use of multiple modes of representation. The juxtaposition of instructional content to provide for multiple perspectives is one of the central themes in Spiro's cognitive flexibility theory (Spiro et al., 1991, 1995).

  7. Providing for the use of multiple modes of representation and learning is supported by the work of researchers such as Cunningham (1992; Duffy & Cunningham, 1996), Honebein (1996), and Gardner (1983, 1985).

  8. 4. Encourage ownership in learning. Much of the work on self-regulated learning (Chapter 9) is consistent with this recommendation. See also Duffy and Cunningham (1996), Honebein (1996), and Lebow (1993).

  9. 5. Nurture self-awareness of the knowledge construction process. Cunningham (1987, 1992) called such self-awareness "reflexivity" and noted that consciously adopting different ways of constructing knowledge enables one to see what is illuminated or hidden by any particular way.

  10. Let us examine each of these constructivists conditions in some detail.

  11. Complex and Relevant Learning Environments.

  12. "Students cannot be expected to learn to deal with complexity unless they have the opportunity to do so" (CTGV, 1991a, p. 36; emphasis theirs).

  13. This bold statement undoubtedly reflects the opinions of most constructivists authors, who further believe that simplifying task for learners will prevent them from learning how to solve the complex problems they will face in real life.

  14. For problem-solving skills to be maximally facilitated, they argue, learners must cope with very complex situations.

  15. Remember from Chapter 5 that Schoenfeld's students believed math problems were virtually unsolvable if they could not be solved in 5 minutes or less (Schoenfeld, 1988).

  16. Experience with only simple problems can lead to such beliefs, whereas experience with more complicated and realistic problems can prevent such erroneous ideas.

  17. What complex problems entail seems to depend largely upon the subject matter within which problem solving and reasoning are being learned.

  18. To a somewhat lesser extent, perhaps, they also depend upon the ages and characteristics of the targeted learners.

  19. The video-based learning environments that the CTGV (1990, 1991a, 1993) developed for mathematical problem solving, for example, contain problems of more than 15 interrelated steps.

  20. All of the information required to solve these problems is incorporated into each video, but the students must decide what information is relevant and how various pieces fit together.

  21. Initially used with fifth and sixth grade students, the videos have apparently been adapted successfully for use with first and second graders (CTGV, 1991b).

  22. Learning environment complexity can also be conceived in terms of both the tools and the content of learning (Perkins, 1991a, 1991b).

  23. With respect to content, much constructivists instruction aims to debunk students’ naïve conceptions or misconceptions, particularly in the areas of science and mathematics.

  24. To do this, situations must make plain the inconsistencies and inadequacies of the learners’ models and “challenge [them] either to construct better models or at least to ponder the merits of alternative models presented by the teacher” (Perkins, 1991b, p. 19).

  25. But what should such situations look like?

  26. This is where the tools of a rich learning environment come in. Specifically, Perkins proposed that “constructivist kits” and “phenomenaria” be widely used in the classroom (1991a; see also Wilson, 1996).

  27. Construction kits enable learners to assemble “not just things such as Tinker Toys, but more abstract entities, such as commands in a program language, creatures in a simulated ecology, or equations in an environment supporting mathematical manipulations” (Perkins, 1991a, p. 19).

  28. So, for example, Legos, learning logs, and software such as Geometric Supposer would be considered construction kits (Wilson, 1996).

  29. Similarly, phenomenaria enable students to observe various phenomena and to manipulate concepts and assumptions within those phenomena.

  30. The popular software series SimCity and SimEarth are good examples of phenomenaria. SimCity is a simulation of real world cities that allows students to explore what it means to build and manage all the various aspects of city life.

  31. Unlike simulations that are carried out for scientific investigation or technical purposes, phenomenaria emphasize the instructional nature of simulations (Wilson, 1996).

  32. An alternative argument for complex learning environments comes from research on how people learn to solve problems in “ill-structured domains” (Spiro et al., 1991, 1995; see also Spiro & Jehng. 1990); Jonassen, 1997, 1999).

  33. Unlike solving an algebraic problem, for example, diagnosing a medical problem depends more on heuristics than on well-formed rules. Furthermore, a doctor (unlike a mathematician) has no proven means for determining whether a diagnosis is correct.

  34. Although a prescribed treatment may appear to be successful in curing the patient, at least tow other possibilities are often equally plausible.

  35. The treatment may be ineffective and the patient got better on his or her own or the treatment effectively cured the problem, but the problem was not what was originally diagnosed.

  36. Doctors must be prepared to accept either of these possibilities if additional evidence seems to warrant it.

  37. Spiro and his colleagues documented the tendency of medical student to oversimplify the concepts and principles comprising diagnostic medicine.

  38. They argue that “instructional focus on general principles with wide scope of application across cases or examples” (Spiro et al., 1991, p. 27) was the cause.

  39. Part of the solution, therefore, should be to retain, in medical instruction, the complexity inherent in this ill-structured domain.

  40. In order to do this, cases should be studied as they really occurred, “not as stripped down ‘textbook examples’ that conveniently illustrate some principle’ (Spiro et al., 1987, p. 181).

  41. In learning about hypertension, then, medical students might best examine multiple case histories of hypertensive patients, so that the full range of their symptoms might be illustrated.

  42. Jonassen (1997, 1999) offered an instructional design model for developing instruction to teach problem solving in both well-structured and ill-structured domains.

  43. With respect to ill-structured problems, he recommended that a context analysis be conducted to lay out the nature of the problem domain and the constraints that might affect problems in the domain.

  44. In a domain such as medical diagnostics, for instance, an increase in malpractice suits could certainly affect doctors’ use of additional tests to verify an initial diagnosis.

  45. Jonassen suggested that these kinds of constraints be introduced during instruction as students pondered case problems.

  46. Sometimes, complex and realistic learning environments are taken to mean the same thing as authentic, or real-world, learning environments.

  47. Certainly, there is value in learners practicing their skills in an authentic performance context, as when young musicians play in an orchestra recital at their school.

  48. But they would have difficulty becoming proficient players if all their practice occurred in that context.

  49. Thus, Anderson, Reder, and Simon (2000) sounded a cautionary note about complex learning situations, echoing Jonassen’s (1999) belief that a restrictive conception of authentic will result in learning environments that are authentic only in a narrow context (p. 221).

More Related