1 / 16

Longitudinal Studies of Children’s Intelligence

Longitudinal Studies of Children’s Intelligence. IQ appears to be relatively stable from age 3 through adulthood. Overall, retest correlations tend to be higher when: The interval is shorter. The older the children.

dong
Télécharger la présentation

Longitudinal Studies of Children’s Intelligence

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Longitudinal Studies ofChildren’s Intelligence • IQ appears to be relatively stable from age 3 through adulthood. • Overall, retest correlations tend to be higher when: • The interval is shorter. • The older the children. • Correlation between IQ in latter preschool and grade school ranges from .6 to .8. Between adolescents, it increases to ~ .8.

  2. Possible Reasons for IQ Stability • Overlap hypothesis - IQ is cumulative. • Overlap between current and past knowledge. • Environmental stability - People tend to remain in the same environments across the developmental life span. • Prerequisite Learning Skills – • Individual retains the same learning tools (e.g., problem-solving methods) from year to year. • Neurological Consistency-The pattern of neurological development remains relatively constant.

  3. Evidence for the Instability of IQ • While overall IQ remains constant across large groups of individuals, specific individuals may exhibit large changes in IQ. • This may be due to changes such as dramatic shifts in environment or individual factors.

  4. Instability of IQ(continued) • California Guidance Study (1948-12 year period): • 59% of children exhibited changes of > 15 points • 37% > 20 or more point • 9% > 30 or more points.

  5. Instability of IQ (continued) • Garber Study (1988) • Generally, children in culturally disadvantaged environments lose IQ points whereas people in culturally enriched environments gain IQ points. • Specifically, examined the relationship between children reared in environments with mothers who had IQ’s either >80 or <80. • No difference, from 13-35 months (avg IQ = 95) but by 14, > 80 mothers = 90, whereas, < 80 mothers = 67.

  6. Instability of IQ (continued) • Instability attributable to changes in: • Parental concern re: education. • Also, home environments characterized by “encouragement…structure” associated with increases in IQ. • Emotional dependency on parents was another factor responsible for IQ loss in childhood. • During school years, IQ increases were associated with high achievement drive, competitive striving, and curiosity about nature.

  7. IQ in Early Childhood • Preschool tests have moderate predictive validity in predicting future IQ. • Between ages 2 and 14, the correlation is about .3. • Infant tests have little predictive validity (best for concurrent validity - relative status now). • Little correlation in scales even with intervals as small as 3 months. • The reason for these low correlations is the change in the construct of IQ.

  8. IQ in Infancy(continued) • The construct of IQ in infancy is made up of largely motor development and visual-perceptional skills. • This differs from how intelligence is defined in school aged children. • In infancy, parental education and other characteristics of home environment are better predictors of subsequent IQ.

  9. Early Childhood IQ (continued) • This speaks to the importance of early intervention programs that provide training in the prerequisite learning skills and the need for parental involvement. • Beyond 18 months, prediction of IQ is improved if tests are combined with family SES information. • However, infant tests may have higher predictive validities within non-normative samples.

  10. Typical IQ instruments • Bayley Tests of Infant Development- 0-42 months. • Stanford-Binet V (2 through adulthood). • Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence (2.5 years to 7.25 years). • Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV • (6 through 16-11 months).

  11. Issues in the Assessment of Adult IQ Findings re: Age and IQ • In Cross-sectional designs, such as that used in Wechsler's standardization samples, IQ declines with age. • Possible explanations include: • Less education • Poorer health • Less familiarity with the information age.

  12. Issues in IQ Assessment (continued) • Longitudinal designs involve testing one cohort (same individuals) at different times. • In contrast to cross-sectional designs, using longitudinal designs, IQ increases with age. • Explanation includes exposure to more information as one grows older.

  13. Issues in IQ Assessment (continued) • In Cross-sequential designs, combination of longitudinal and cross-sectional using a time-lag design. • A time-lag design provides that same-age cohorts are tested at different time periods (e.g., 40-year-olds tested at 1970 and 1980). • That is, 20 years tested in 1940 are compared to themselves in 1970 and 20 year olds tested in 1970.

  14. Cross-Sequential Findings • Older cohorts have higher IQ's from when they were tested earlier (IQ increases over time). • However, when different age groups were compared to one another at times (20 y.o. in 1950 vs. 20 y.o. in 1970), no significant differences emerged. • This suggests that experiential factors, not age itself, leads to changes in IQ.

  15. Conclusions re: Changes in Adult IQ • True age decrements do not begin until well after age 60. • One exception is speeded tests which do decrease with age. • Overall changes in IQ in older age are largely related to health status. • That is, elderly individuals in poorer health do less well on tests of intelligence.

  16. Conclusions (continued) • Test norms require frequent updating. • Experiential, cultural, variables must be taken into account in interpreting test scores. • IQ scores do measure cultural factors.

More Related