1 / 34

Brief Notes on Theoretical Neuroscience

Brief Notes on Theoretical Neuroscience. April 14, 2010 Gabriel Kreiman http://klab.tch.harvard.edu. Outline. Why theoretical neuroscience? Single neuron models Network models Algorithms and methods for data analysis A sample of a few computational studies (visual recognition).

idola
Télécharger la présentation

Brief Notes on Theoretical Neuroscience

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Brief Notes on Theoretical Neuroscience April 14, 2010 Gabriel Kreiman http://klab.tch.harvard.edu

  2. Outline • Why theoretical neuroscience? • Single neuron models • Network models • Algorithms and methods for data analysis • A sample of a few computational studies (visual recognition)

  3. Further reading This will NOT be an exhaustive presentation of work in Theoretical Neuroscience… • Abbott and Dayan. Theoretical Neuroscience - Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems [2001] (ISBN 0-262-04199-5). MIT Press. • Koch. Biophysics of computation [1999] (ISBN 0-19-510491-9). Oxford University Press. • Hertz, Krogh, and Palmer, Introduction to the theory of neural computation. [1991] (ISBN 0-20151560-1). Santa Fe Institute Studies in the Sciences of Complexity.

  4. Why bother with models? • Quantitative models force us to think about and formalize hypotheses and assumptions • Models can integrate and summarize observations across experiments, resolutions and laboratories • A good model can lead to (non-intuitive) experimental predictions • A quantitative model, implemented through simulations, can be useful from an engineering viewpoint (e.g. face recognition) • A model can point to important missing data, critical information and decisive experiments

  5. Do I have to be a “professional theoretician” to build a model? • No! • There are plenty of excellent computational papers written by experimentalists… • A very brief sample: • Laurent G (2002) Olfactory Network Dynamics and the coding of multidimensional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3:884-895.- Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Movshon JA (1997) Linearity and normalization in simple cells of the macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 17:8621-8644.- Brincat S, Connor C (2006) Dynamic Shape Synthesis in Posterior Inferior Temporal Cortex. Neuron 49:17-24- Blake R (1989) A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychological Review 96:145-167.- Hubel, D.H. and T.N. Wiesel, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol, 1962. 160:106-54. • Scientists always build models (even if the models are not quantitative and are not implemented through computational simulations). There is no such thing as a “model-free” experiment…

  6. A model for orientation tuning in simple cells A feed-forward model for orientation selectivity in V1 (by no means the only model) Hubel and Wiesel. J. Physiology (1962)

  7. More anatomical complexity – Similar math Douglas and Martin 2004 Felleman and Van Essen 1991

  8. Outline • Why theoretical neuroscience? • Single neuron models • Network models • Algorithms and methods • A sample of a few computational studies

  9. A nested family of single neuron models Filter operations Integrate-and-fire circuit Hodgkin-Huxley units Multi-compartmental models Biological accuracy Lack of analytical solutions Computational complexity Spines, channels

  10. Geometrically accurate models vs. spherical cows with point masses A central question in Theoretical Neuroscience: What is the “right” level of abstraction?

  11. The leaky integrate-and-fire model • Lapicque 1907 • Below threshold, the voltage is governed by: • A spike is fired when V(t)>Vthr (and V(t) is reset) • A refractory period tref is imposed after a spike. • Simple and fast. • Does not consider spike-rate adaptation, multiple compartments, sub-ms biophysics, neuronal geometry Vrest=-65 mV Vth =-50 mV Τm = 10 ms Rm = 10 MΩ Line = I&F model Circles = cortex first 2 spikes adapted

  12. The Hodgkin-Huxley Model where: im = membrane current V = voltage L = leak channel K = potassium channel Na = sodium channel g = conductances (e.g. gNa=120 mS/cm2; gK=36 mS/cm2; gL=0.3 mS/cm2) E = reversal potentials (e.g. ENa=115mV, EK=-12 mV, EL = 10.6 mV) n, m, h = “gating variables”, n=n(t), m=m(t), h=h(t) Hodgkin, A. L., and Huxley, A. F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. Journal of Physiology 117, 500-544.

  13. Outline • Why theoretical neuroscience? • Single neuron models • Network models • Algorithms and methods • A sample of a few computational studies

  14. From neurons to circuits Single neurons can perform many interesting and important computations (e.g. Gabbiani et al (2002). Multiplicative computation in a visual neuron sensitive to looming. Nature 420, 320-324) Neurons are not isolated. They are part of circuits. A typical cortical neuron receives input from ~104 other neurons. It is not always trivial to predict circuit-level properties from single neuron properties. There could be interesting properties emerging at the network level.

  15. Circuits – some basic definitions Notes: Connectivity does not need to be all-to-all There are excitatory neurons and inhibitory neurons (and many types of inhibitory neurons) Most models assume balance between excitation and inhibition Most models do not include layers and the anatomical separation of forward and back pathways There are many more recurrent+feedback connections than feed-forward connections (the opposite is true about models…)

  16. Firing rate network models – A simple feedforward circuit • Time scales > ~ 1 ms • Analytic calculations in some cases • Fewer free parameters than spiking models • Easier/faster to simulate Is = total synaptic currentN = total number of inputswb = synaptic weightsKs(t) = synaptic kernelub = input firing rates if F can be a sigmoid function Or a threshold linear function:

  17. Learning from examples – The perceptron Imagine that we want to classify the inputs u into two groups “+1” and “-1” Training examples: {um,vm} Perceptron learning rule Linear separability: can attain zero error Cross-validation: use separate training and test data There are several more sophisticated learning algorithms

  18. Learning from examples – Gradient descent Now imagine that v is a real value (as opposed to binary) We want to choose the weights so that the output approximates some function h(s) Move along the gradient of the error between the desired output and the current output

  19. Outline • Why theoretical neuroscience? • Single neuron models • Network models • Algorithms and methods • A sample of a few computational studies

  20. Some examples of computational algorithms and methods Different techniques for time-frequency analysis of neural signals (e.g. Pesaran et al 2002, Fries et al 2001) Spike sorting (e.g. Lewicki 1998, Quian Quiroga et al 2005) Machine learning approaches to decoding neuronal responses (e.g. Hung et al 2005, Wilson et al 1993, Musallam et al 2004) Information theory (e.g. Abbott et al 1996, Bialek et al 1991) Neural coding (e.g. Gabbiani et al 1998, Bialek et al 1991) Definition of spatio-temporal receptive fields, phenomenological models, measures of neuronal synchrony, spike train statistics

  21. Outline • Why theoretical neuroscience? • Single neuron models • Network models • Algorithms and methods • A sample of a few computational studies

  22. Predicting spikes in the retina • s(t) = visual stimulus • F(t) = linear filter • “*” = convolution • g(t)= “generator” potential • = threshold • r(t) = firing event (1 when g(t)>) Keat J, Reinagel P, Reid RC, Meister M (2001) Predicting every spike: a model for the responses of visual neurons. Neuron 30:803-817.

  23. Predicting spikes in the retina h(t)=g(t)+P(t) P(t)=Bexp(-t/t) Keat J, Reinagel P, Reid RC, Meister M (2001) Predicting every spike: a model for the responses of visual neurons. Neuron 30:803-817.

  24. Predicting spikes in the retina Two “noise” sources are added to account for trial-to-trial variability: a(t) = gaussian noise at the generator potential b(t) = random fluctuations in the feedback potential P(t) Keat J, Reinagel P, Reid RC, Meister M (2001) Predicting every spike: a model for the responses of visual neurons. Neuron 30:803-817.

  25. The “blue brain” modeling project • http://bluebrain.epfl.ch • IBM’s Blue gene supercomputer • “Reverse engineer” the brain in a “biologically accurate” way • November 2007 milestone: 30 million synapses in “precise” locations to model a neocortical column • Compartmental simulations for neurons • Needs another supercomputer for visualization (10,000 neurons, high quality mesh, 1 billion triangles, 100 Gb) QUESTION: What is the “right” level of abstraction needed to understand the function of cortical circuitry?

  26. An object can cast an infinite number of projections on the retina

  27. A brute force approach to object recognition Task: Recognize the handwritten “A” A “brute force” solution: - Use templates for each letter - Use multiple scales for each template - Use multiple positions for each template - Use multiple rotations for each template - Etc. Problems with this approach: - Large amount of storage for each object - No extrapolation, no intelligent learning - Need to learn about each object under each condition

  28. A non-exhaustive list of biological object recognition models • Some common themes across multiple models: • Hierarchical structure • Increased “receptive field” size • Increased complexity in shape preferences • Increased invariance K. Fukushima, Neocognitron: a self organizing neural network model for a mechanism of pattern recognition unaffected by shift in position. Biological Cybernetics, 1980. 36: 193-202. Y. LeCun, L. Bottou, Y. Bengioand P. Haffner, Gradient-based learning applied to document recognition. Proc of the IEEE, 1998. 86: 2278-2324. G. Wallis and E.T. Rolls, Invariant face and object recognition in the visual system. Progress in Neurobiology, 1997. 51: 167-94. B. Mel, SEEMORE: Combining color, shape and texture histogramming in a neurally inspired approach to visual object recognition. Neural Computation, 1997. 9: 777. B.A. Olshausen, C.H. Anderson and D.C. Van Essen, A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information. J Neurosci, 1993. 13: 4700-19. M. Riesenhuberand T. Poggio, Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 1999. 2: 1019-1025. G. Deco and E.T. Rolls, A neurodynamical cortical model of visual attention and invariant object recognition. Vision Res, 2004. 44: 621-42. P. Foldiak, Learning Invariance from Transformation Sequences. Neural Computation, 1991. 3: 194-200.

  29. Neocognitron Retinotopically arranged connections between layers Feature extracting “S” cells C-cells performing a local “OR” operation Increasing buildup of position tolerance Unsupervised learning in S layers Fukushima K. (1980) Neocognitron: a self organizing neural network model for a mechanism fo pattern recognition unaffected by shift in position. Biological Cybernetics 36, 193-202

  30. Recognizing objects by part decomposition Biederman (1987) Psychological Review

  31. Object recognition by alignment to prototypes Prototype Alignment of 3 points to the prototype (black arrows) Note: some points may not align (red ellipses) Ullman (1996) High-level vision

  32. Invariance in visual object recognition Several computational models for rotation invariance rely on building 3D object models Here, recognition is based on learning from few perspective views Generalized radial basis functions ti = centers ci = coefficients G = basis function (e.g. gaussian) Poggio T, Edelman S (1990) A network that learns to recognize 3D objects. Nature 343:263-266.

  33. Invariance in visual object recognition Poggio T, Edelman S (1990) A network that learns to recognize 3D objects. Nature 343:263-266.

  34. Computer vision Wang et al CVPR 2006 • Goal: “generic” object recognition • Challenge: object transformations, intra-class variability for categorization • e.g. Caltech 101 dataset, 30-800 exemplars/category • Learning generative visual models.L. Fei-Fei, R. Fergus, and P. Perona. CVPR 2004Shape Matching and Object Recognition using Low Distortion Correspondence. Alexander C. Berg, Tamara L. Berg, JitendraMalik. CVPR 2005 • The Pyramid Match Kernel:DiscriminativeClassification with Sets of Image Features. K. Graumanand T. Darrell. ICCV) 2005. • Combining Generative Models and Fisher Kernels Holub, AD. Welling, M. Perona, P. ICCV 2005 • Exploiting Unlabelled Data for Hybrid Object Classification.Holub, AD. Welling, M. Perona, P. NIPS 2005 Workshop in Inter-Class Transfer. • Object Recognition with Features Inspired by Visual Cortex. T. Serre, L. Wolf and T. Poggio. CVPR 2005.

More Related