1 / 59

Sharon A. Reeve , Ph.D., BCBA Oct. 14, 2003 Presentation at Queens College

Effects of Video Modeling, Prompting, and Reinforcement Strategies on I ncreasing A Generalized Repertoire of Prosocial Behavior in Children with Autism. Sharon A. Reeve , Ph.D., BCBA Oct. 14, 2003 Presentation at Queens College. Importance of a Definition.

sarah
Télécharger la présentation

Sharon A. Reeve , Ph.D., BCBA Oct. 14, 2003 Presentation at Queens College

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Effects of Video Modeling, Prompting, and Reinforcement Strategies onIncreasing A Generalized Repertoire of Prosocial Behavior in Children with Autism Sharon A. Reeve, Ph.D., BCBA Oct. 14, 2003 Presentation at Queens College

  2. Importance of a Definition • Researchers have been inconsistent in defining prosocial behavior • We need to identify antecedent and consequential stimuli to effectively define prosocial behavior • We can then treat it as a type of operant behavior and subsequently teach it (Novak, 1996)

  3. Broad Definition of Prosocial Behavior • Any act intending to benefit another such as responses associated with helping, cooperating, sharing, care giving, taking turns, friendliness, affection, empathy, and/or sympathy • Distinction among categories is ambiguous • Observed in children of typical development as early as 1 ½ to 3 years of age • Children have been observed offering to help another person who is injured by offering a toy or trying to comfort. (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998)

  4. Prosocial Behavior is Learned • Prosocial behavior is primarily learned by imitation of children of typical development • In contrast children with autism are unlikely to imitate one engaging in prosocial behavior • Children of typical development are more likely to initiate prosocial behavior than children with autism. • Equally to children with disabilities and children of typical development • Children with autism were still unlikely to imitate the prosocial behavior. (Honig & McCarron, 1988)

  5. Importance of Prosocial Behavior in Children • Increased perception of social competence (Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Mur­phy, Wosinski, Polazzi, Carlo, & Juhnke, 1996; Peterson, Ridley-Johnson, & Carter, 1984).  • Increased frequency in engagement in positive social interactions with peers (Farver & Branstetter, 1994; Howes & Farver, 1987). • Increased likelihood of having many close friends and a best friend (Farver & Branstetter, 1994; Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990).

  6. Negative Outcomes of Prosocial Behavior Deficit in Children with Autism • Can be a source of frustration and distress to those who interact with a person with autism (Harris, Handleman, & Alessandri, 1990). • Parents, peers, and teachers may be discouraged from attempting to interact with the children (Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons, & Long 1973). • Further reduces the opportunities for learning (Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons, & Long, 1973).

  7. Negative Outcomes of Prosocial Behavior Deficit in Children with Autism • Unlikely to have friends or sustained interactions with peers (Sigman, 1998) • Perceived as less socially competent, whether valid or based on biased expectations (Center & Wascom, 1986)

  8. Theories for Deficits in Prosocial Behavior • Lack “theory-of-mind” (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) • Early deficit in imitation (Rogers & Pennington, 1991) • Result of an interaction between deficient innate structures that play a role in processing of emotional input and a lack of social experience (Bemporad et al., 1987)

  9. Theories for Deficits in Prosocial Behavior • Various cognitive deficits (Gillberg, 1992) • Failure to discriminate and respond to social cues in environment (Morrison & Bellack, 1981)

  10. Increasing Prosocial Behavior in Children with Autism Under Specific Training Conditions (No Generalization Measures) • Kohler, Strain, Hoyson, Davis, Donina, & Rapp (1995) • a comprehensive intervention increased prosocial behavior between children with autism and their peers

  11. Increasing Prosocial Behavior in Children with Autism Under Specific Training Conditions (No Generalization Measures) • Kamps, Leonard, Vernon, Dugan, Delquadri, Gershon, Wade, & Folk (1992) • social skills training for students with autism conducted concurrently with non-handicapped peers was an effective procedure for increasing: • frequency of social interactions • amount of time engaged in prosocial behavior • duration of each social interaction in children with autism

  12. Increasing Prosocial Behavior in Children with AutismUnder Specific Training Conditions (With Generalization Measures) • Strain, Kerr, & Ragland (1979) • indicated that both intervention techniques increased each child with autism’s prosocial behavior • Neither technique, however, produced increase in prosocial behavior during generalization sessions • Charlop & Walsh (1986) • time-delay procedure was effective in teaching children with autism to respond with “I love (like) you” in response to a hug • target behavior generalized across settings for all children, but across persons and settings for only one child

  13. Increasing Prosocial Behavior in Children with AutismUnder Specific Training Conditions (With Generalization Measures) • Harris, Handleman, & Alessandri (1990) • Taught to offer assistance to a person who expressed an inability to complete a task • adolescents given instructions as to how to help • all three adolescents showed an increase in their offers of assistance as training progressed • generalization occurred across settings, people, and situations during only a limited number of trials

  14. Strategies that Increase Generalization • Teaching multiple exemplars of target behavior using common stimuli (Balsam, 1988; Stokes & Baer, 1977) • Teaching generalized imitative repertoire (Harris et al., 1990; Poulson & Kymissis, 1988). • Use of video modeling (Charlop, Schreibman, & Tyron, 1983; Haring, Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-Conway, 1987) 

  15. Purpose of Present Study • To determine extent to which children with autism can learn to engage in both verbal and motor prosocial responses commonly labeled as helping • “Helping” was selected as prosocial response because it results in longer interactions than other prosocial responses • To determine extent to which helping responses generalized from training to novel situations in which there was an opportunity to engage in helping behavior

  16. Method Participants • Four children with autism (Irene, Tom, Eddie, and Nathan) who attended classes at Institute for Educational Achievement (IEA) • They ranged in age from 5-6 years

  17. Method Setting • Most experimental sessions took place in small classroom at IEA • Approximately once every two weeks, sessions were conducted in staff room at IEA • To assess occurrence of helping responses in a new setting, pre- and post-intervention measures were taken in children’s regular school classrooms at IEA

  18. Definition of “Helping” • Helping: a child with autism engaging in a problem-solving activity with an adult. • For each problem-solving activity, three different stimulus components were used to signal to the child that a specific helping response should be emitted: • non-verbal • verbal • affective discriminative stimuli

  19. Eight Possible Experimenter-Defined Categories of Helping • Cleaning • Replacing Broken Materials  • Picking up Objects  • Sorting materials  • Locating Objects • Carrying Objects  • Putting Items Away  • Setting Up an Activity

  20. Category Structure

  21. Criterion that Defined a Mastery Level of Performance • Correct combined verbal and motor helping response on at least 94% (15/16) of the total number of training trials for four consecutive sessions. 

  22. Categories for Each Child

  23. Assignment of Trials (for Tom)

  24. Baseline & Treatment Trials • Baseline • Both training and probe trials presented • Neither trial type associated with treatment or reinforcement • Token reinforcement and verbal praise provided only for on-task behavior • Treatment • Both training and probe trials presented • Training trials associated with treatment • Probe trials not associated with treatment

  25. Teaching Procedure Presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli (non-verbal & verbal)  Incorrect Verbal and/or Motor Response by child  Presentation of Video Model  Re-presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli  Incorrect Verbal and/or Motor Response by child  Presentation of Motor and/or Verbal Prompts  Re-presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli  Correct Verbal and Motor Responses by child  Reinforcement (token + praise)

  26. Non-Helping Verbal and Non-Verbal Discriminative Stimuli

  27. Additional Strategies Used to Promote Generalization • Treatment sessions were conducted once every eight sessions in the staff room (not the typical experimental session room) • Treatment sessions were conducted once every 10 sessions by a secondary experimenter (not the primary experimenter)

  28. Pre- and Post-Intervention Measures • For each child, three pre-intervention sessions were conducted before treatment was introduced.  • Three post-intervention sessions were conducted after all participants had achieved mastery criterion

  29. Counterbalanced Assignment of Additional Categories

  30. Pre- and Post-Intervention Measures • Measures 1-3 • Combination of novel trials, probe trials, and training trial types   • Conducted in the child’s regular school classroom with their regular school instructor.   • Measures 4-6 • Combination of novel trials, probe trials, and training trial types • Conducted in the child’s regular school classroom by the primary experimenter  • Measure 7 • Used trials from the two novel categories • Conducted in the experimental setting by the primary experimenter

  31. Social Validity Measures • Measure 1 • Used to assess increase in each child’s prosocial behavior from baseline to treatment. • Undergraduate raters were asked: • “In which of the two video-taped episodes (the first or the second) did the child appear to engage in more prosocial behavior?” • Measure 2 • Used to assess whether the prosocial behavior emitted by the children in this study was similar to the behavior emitted by their age-matched peers. • Undergraduate raters were asked: • “Was appropriate prosocial behavior used by this child?”

  32. Interobserver Agreement • Obtained for the percentage of trials that contained a correct or incorrect helping response for each child.   • Obtained for the frequency of occasions in which each child emitted a helping response during non-helping episodes.   • Obtained for the accuracy of presentation of the nonverbal, verbal, and affective discriminative stimuli for all trial types across all experimental conditions and children.

  33. Percentage of Trials in Which Video Presentation Occasioned a Correct Helping Response on Subsequent Presentation of the Discriminative Stimuli

  34. Total Number of Occasions in Which Each ChildEngaged in a Non-Contextual Helping Response

  35. Mean Percentage of Correct Helping Responses Collapsed Across All Seven Post-Intervention Measures

  36. Number of Sessions Required to Achieve a Mastery Level of Performance During Untrained-category Probe Trials and Trained-Category Probe Trials

  37. Results of Social Validity Measures • Measure 1 • indicated that the children with autism’s responses in treatment episodes were more helpful than those in baseline episodes • Measure 2 • indicated no difference between prosocial behavior of the children with autism and age-matched peers of typical development

  38. Conclusions • Results of present study suggest that social programs for children with autism should include training in prosocial behavior • Systematic application of video modeling, prompting, and reinforcement taught children to use both motor and verbal helping responses in training and novel situations

  39. Conclusions • Children demonstrated a generalized repertoire of helping behavior as evidenced by emitting appropriate helping behavior in presence of novel verbal and nonverbal discriminative stimuli drawn from novel categories of helping

  40. Benefits When a Child with Autism Engages in Prosocial Behavior • Children who engage in prosocial behavior tend to be viewed by adults as more socially competent (Eisenberg et al., 1996; Peterson, Ridley-Johnson, & Carter, 1984). • An adult may be more likely to interact with the child with autism who engages in prosocial behavior (Charlop & Walsh, 1986; Harris et al., 1990).

  41. Benefits When a Child with Autism Engages in Prosocial Behavior • Increased social interactions provided for child with autism may result in additional access to social reinforcement (Lovaas et al., 1973; Lovaas, 1981) • Engagement in prosocial behavior may lessen deficits in social behavior prevalent in diagnosis of autism (Wing, 1988)

  42. Future Areas of Research • Define most efficient way to teach a generalized repertoire of helping behavior • Use more natural teaching paradigms, such as incidental teaching (Hart & Risley, 1975), to further facilitate generalization of prosocial behavior from training to novel situations • Identify minimum levels of prerequisite skills that children with autism need to emit before they are able to consistently engage in prosocial behavior

  43. Future Areas of Research • Determine whether early intervention efforts for children with autism that include prosocial behavior training with peers would produce a generalized imitative repertoire of prosocial behavior • Should include follow-up training and maintenance of newly acquired prosocial behavior emitted by children with autism

  44. Social Skills: ProSocial Behavior • Examples of skill acquisition programs • Helping • Perspective taking

  45. “Helping” • Skill Domain: Social & Peer Interaction Skills • Category: ProSocial Behavior • Operational Definition: • Within 5 seconds of the discriminative stimuli (SD) Johnny says “Can I help”AND engages in a problem-solving activity with another person . Examples of problem-solving activities are further defined as teaching sets. • Data are collected minimally weekly and are summarized as percentage of opportunities in which Johnny effectively engaged in a helping response • During data collection, no prompts are used.

  46. “Helping” • Teaching Sets with Specific response definitions • Cleaning • The child places a cloth in contact with a surface and engages in either back-and-forth or circular arm movements until the adult stops making that same motion. • Replacing Broken Materials  • Picking up Objects  • Sorting materials  • Locating Objects • Carrying Objects  • Putting Items Away  • Setting Up an Activity

  47. “Helping” • Discriminative Stimulus: • Non-verbal: various motor movements depending on the set • Verbal: exclamation • Affective: facial expression • Criterion for Advancement: • Engaging in helping on at least 90% of the opportunities for two consecutive sessions.

  48. “Helping”

  49. “Helping”: Teaching Procedure Presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli (non-verbal, verbal affective)  Incorrect Verbal and/or Motor Response by child  Presentation of Video Model  Re-presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli  Incorrect Verbal and/or Motor Response by child  Presentation of Motor and/or Verbal Prompts  Re-presentation of Live Discriminative Stimuli  Correct Verbal and Motor Responses by child  Reinforcement (token + praise)

More Related