1 / 37

Experimental evidence for product-oriented generalizations (or not)

Experimental evidence for product-oriented generalizations (or not). Vsevolod Kapatsinski Indiana University Dept. of Linguistics Cognitive Science Program Speech Research Laboratory vkapatsi@indiana.edu http://mypage.iu.edu/~vkapatsi/. Product-oriented vs. source-oriented generalizations.

Télécharger la présentation

Experimental evidence for product-oriented generalizations (or not)

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Experimental evidence for product-oriented generalizations(or not) Vsevolod Kapatsinski Indiana University Dept. of Linguistics Cognitive Science Program Speech Research Laboratory vkapatsi@indiana.edu http://mypage.iu.edu/~vkapatsi/

  2. Product-oriented vs. source-oriented generalizations • Bybee (2001:126) “Generative rules express source-oriented generalizations. That is, they act on a specific input to change it in well-defined ways into an output of a certain form. Many, if not all, schemas are product-oriented rather than source-oriented. A product-oriented schema generalizes over forms of a specific category, but does not specify how to derive that category from some other.” Source oriented: k]sg ti]pl Product-oriented: ‘plurals must end in ti’

  3. Present study • Given an artificial lexicon and a particular training paradigm what generalizations do the learners extract?

  4. The paradigm(Bybee & Newman 1995)

  5. The artificial languages Two plural suffixes –i and -a If –i attached to a velar ({k;g}), the velar changes to an alveopalatal This is velar palatalization

  6. Velar palatalization The process: • k  t /_i Productivity: • p(k  ti) / ( p(k  ti) + p(kki) ) Coding scheme: BLUE – velar palatalization applies RED – velar palatalization fails

  7. Research question • Does the productivity of velar palatalization differ in the BLUE language and the RED language? • Depends on your model of grammar.

  8. The possible grammars /62

  9. Triggers velar palatalization Does not compete with anything Equally supported in both languages Non-competing rules BLUE = RED e.g., Hale and Reiss 2008, Plag 2003

  10. Triggers velar palatalization Equally supported in both languages Simple positive product-oriented generalizations BLUE = RED Bybee & Slobin 1982, Bybee & Moder 1983, Bybee 2001

  11. Triggers velar palatalization /ki/ less expected in the blue language  its absence is less notable Negative product-oriented generalizations BLUE < RED

  12. Triggers velar palatalization Competes with Competition stronger in red Competing weighted rules BLUE > RED Albright & Hayes 2003 Iff the choice between the rules is stochastic.

  13. Triggers velar palatalization More reliable in blue Conditional product-oriented generalizations BLUE > RED Aslin et al. 1998

  14. Result BLUERED

  15. Results Non-competing rules Simple positive product-oriented Negative product-oriented Competing weighted rules Conditional product-oriented 100% 30 * BLUE RED

  16. Individual subject data

  17. Competing weighted rules {p;b;t;d} {p;b;t;d} Albright & Hayes 2003

  18. Results *** Competing weighted rules ANCOVA: This correlation is significant F(1,27)=14.23, p<.001, while Language is not, F(1,27)=.082, p>.5). The predicted explanatory variable accounts for all the variance in velar palatalization rate attributable to the artificial language 22

  19. The competing rules look good.Can we pit them against (conditional) product-oriented generalizations directly?

  20. Conditional product-oriented generalizations

  21. Competing weighted rules

  22. Competing weighted rules • r(33) = -.49, p=.003

  23. Competing weighted rules (Conditional) product-oriented The addition of tti hurts vel.pal t(33)=2.88, p=.007

  24. Something that looks product-oriented but isn’t RED BLUE Result: All subjects attach –i rather than –a to singulars ending in {t;dʒ} In the Blue Language even more than to singulars ending in {k;g} H: Because both languages have plurals ending in {t;dʒ}i, not {t;dʒ}a. A product-oriented schema? ‘Plurals must end in {t;dʒ}i’.

  25. Why not? • If • {t;dʒ} {t;dʒ}i because of ‘Plurals must end in {t;dʒ}i’, and this is the schema that does {k;g} {t;dʒ}i, • Then • there should be a positive correlation between rate of {k;g} {t;dʒ}i and rate of {t;dʒ} {t;dʒ}i • But r=-.03, n.s.

  26. It’s categorization of source forms. Why more –i with {t;dʒ} than with {k;g}? Subjects have a bias against stem changes. The more a subject attaches –i to velars, the more s/he attaches it to alveopalatals.

  27. Prior experimental evidence for product-oriented generalizations • Frequent output patterns get ‘overused’, being derived from inputs in ways that are not attested in the lexicon, e.g., [vn]  [v] (Bybee & Moder, 1983). (Also see Köpcke 1988, Lobben 1991, Wang and Derwing 1994, Albright and Hayes 2003) • H: because the subjects have generalized ‘past tense forms must end in []’

  28. An alternative interpretation • H’: The production of an output primes sublexical chunks occurring in that output. • Lobben (1991) • “the plurals [that don’t obey the rules but all end in ooCii] are appearing concentrated and subsequently… and… this is a typical characteristic of all other plural patterns’ (Lobben 1991:173), • ‘[In this example] the second syllable of the singular is left out in the plural form, which never happens with real nouns… The surrounding… plurals, two preceding and seven following… are trisyllabic [in accordance with source-oriented rules]. This… provides an explanation as to why the plural [in this example], which, if produced according to the rule… , would have four syllables, is made to have three syllables in a very unorthodox way’ (Lobben 1991:182) • Presupposition: the output is more salient than the input  chunks from the output are more likely to persist and be reused than chunks from the input

  29. Are products more salient than sources? • During training, subjects repeated the word pairs they heard. • Subjects have a bias against stem changes  If the product is more salient, they should tend to erroneously make the shape of the singular fit the shape of the plural.  If the source is more salient, the plural should fit the singular.

  30. Products are more salient χ2=28.9, p<.0001 t  ti k  ki k  ti repeated as

  31. In this AGL paradigm (Bybee & Newman 1995), Learners extract competing rules The outcome of competition is influenced by reliability or type frequency (Albright and Hayes 2003, Pierrehumbert 2006) The choice between rules is stochastic No evidence for product-oriented generalizations. Future work: Role of the training paradigm. Summary 35

  32. Product forms are more salient than source forms. Thus creating a product may prime chunks and patterns that occur in that product for immediate reuse. This product priming may result in ‘overuse’ of frequent product patterns (found by Bybee & Moder 1983, Koepcke 1988, Lobben 1991, Wang & Derwing 1994, Albright & Hayes, 2003). If long-lasting, it may also result in the emergence of product-oriented schemas over time. Summary 36

  33. References Albright, A., and B. Hayes. 2003. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition, 90, 119-61. Aslin, R. N., J. R. Saffran, & E. L. Newport. 1998. Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9, 321-4. Bybee, J. L. 2001. Phonology and language use. CUP. Bybee, J. L., & C. L. Moder. 1983. Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 59, 251-70. Bybee, J. L., & J. E. Newman. 1995. Are stem changes as natural as affixes? Linguistics, 33, 633-54. Bybee, J. L., & D. I. Slobin. 1982. Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past. Language 58: 265-89. Hale, M., & C. Reiss. 2008. The phonological enterprise. OUP. Köpcke, K.-M. 1988. Schemas in German plural formation. Lingua, 74, 303-35. Lobben, M. 1991. Pluralization of Hausa nouns, viewed from psycholinguistic experiments and child language data. M.Phil Thesis, University of Oslo. Pierrehumbert, J. B. 2006. The statistical basis of an unnatural alternation. In Laboratory Phonology 8, 81-107. Mouton de Gruyter. Plag, I. 1999. Word formation in English. Mouton de Gruyter. Wang, H. S., & B. L. Derwing. 1994. Some vowel schemas in three English morphological classes: Experimental evidence. In M. Y. Chen & O. C. L. Tseng, eds. In honor of Professor William S.-Y. Wang: Interdisciplinary studies on language and language change, 561-75. Taipei: Pyramid Press.

More Related