1 / 37

Mindsets and Motivation

Mindsets and Motivation. Claudia Mueller, PhD, MD Division of Pediatric Surgery. Framework. Based on work by psychologist Carol Dweck, PhD Dweck et al have identified a set of “implicit theories” or “mindsets” that individuals use to analyze and interpret the world

arion
Télécharger la présentation

Mindsets and Motivation

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Mindsets and Motivation Claudia Mueller, PhD, MD Division of Pediatric Surgery

  2. Framework • Based on work by psychologist Carol Dweck, PhD • Dweck et al have identified a set of “implicit theories” or “mindsets” that individuals use to analyze and interpret the world • These theories are based on two different assumptions that people make about the malleability of personal attributes

  3. Implicit theories • “Entity” theory • The belief that a personal attribute is fixed and nonmalleable • “Incremental” theory • The belief that an attribute is a malleable quality that is subject to change and development • Beliefs can be applied in various domains: intelligence, morality, athletics

  4. Implicit theories 2 • Entity/incremental category determined by responses to statements • Implicit theories • Intelligence • You can learn new things, but you can’t really change your basic intelligence. • Morality • A person’s moral character is something very basic about them and it can’t be changed very much.

  5. Mindset statements • Your intelligence is something very basic about you that you can’t change very much. • You can learn new things, but you can’t really change how intelligent you are. • No matter how much intelligence you have, you can always change it quite a bit. • You can always substantially change how intelligent you are.

  6. Implicit theories 3 • Entity vs. Incremental theories influence • Judgments • Entity see failures as indicative of stable, low ability • Goals • Performance (look good, avoid failure) vs. mastery(learn material, may have to fail to do so) • Response to setbacks • Entity give up in face of challenge • Theories create “mindsets” which influence behavior • Independent of actual intelligence, morality, athletic ability

  7. Intelligence theory • Entity view of intelligence as fixed stable trait • Intelligence is reflected by individual outcomes • Performance goals: challenge is threatening • Failure is devastating (I’m dumb) • Incremental view of intelligence as able to be developed • Individual outcomes not representative of whole • Learning goals: challenge is informative • Failure is motivating (I need to work harder)

  8. Theory development • Likely related to feedback received throughout life • Parents, teachers, peers • Some studies have looked at feedback after failure • My work has focused on feedback after success

  9. Feedback studies • Does feedback change how children respond to challenge? • Can different types of praise lead children to develop different goals and theories (mindsets)? • Conventional wisdom • Praise in any form is good • Praise after success • builds self esteem • protects against disappointment after failure

  10. Type of praise • Praise for: • Fixed ability = “smartness” • Malleable effort = “hard work” • Would children respond differently based on the type of praise given? • Conventional wisdom: no • Hypothesis: yes

  11. Dissertation • Series of 6 studies with fifth-graders assessed for their work on geometric puzzles (Mueller & Dweck) • >500 participants • public and private schools • Midwest, Northeast • Dweck lab, Psychology Dept, Columbia University

  12. Experimental Design Puzzle 1 (Success) Praise manipulation Puzzle 2 (Failure) Measures Puzzle Enjoyment Task Persistence Failure Attributions Puzzle 3 (Test)

  13. Puzzles • Raven’s Progressive Matrices • John C. Raven, Oxford Psychologists Press

  14. Praise • Wow, you did very well on these problems. You got – right. That’s a really high score. • Ability • You must be smart at these problems. • Effort • You must have worked hard at these problems.

  15. Failure • Children told they scored “a lot worse” on second set of puzzles • Post-failure assessments • How well they did • Enjoyment • Persistence • Attributions • I didn’t work hard enough • I’m not good enough at the problems • I’m not smart enough • I didn’t have enough time

  16. Psychological impact of praise • Children praised for effort • Attributed failure to lack of trying • Enjoyed puzzles more • More likely to persist • Children praised for ability • Attributed failure to lack of ability • Enjoyed the puzzles less • Gave up sooner • Content of praise matters

  17. Puzzle Performance

  18. Additional findings • Ability praise • Performance goals • Entity belief of intelligence • Sought information on performance not mastery • Misrepresented score to other kids • Effort praise: learning, incremental theory

  19. Conclusions • Praise delivers message • Praise for ability after success • Poor performance = low ability • Avoid challenge • Praise for effort after success • Poor performance = work harder • Embrace challenge as opportunity for growth • Feedback leads to “mindset” that determines behavior

  20. My research paradigm • Propose psychological framework of children’s illness that uses their theories of health to predict adherence and outcomes • Develop scale to identify these health theories • Test scale in healthy children • Study theories and behaviors in chronically ill adolescents • Develop feedback messages to improve both adherence and outcomes in pediatric populations

  21. Response to disease • Little is known about the psychological dimensions of children’s disease • Children respond very differently to stressful hospital settings • Some crumble and regress; others thrive and adapt • “Mindset” of illness • Independent of illness severity • “Mindset” can be created by theory which may determine adherence and, ultimately, response to disease

  22. Translation to clinical setting • Illness is a challenge • Hospitalized children behave similarly to children in schools • Do children perceive health as they do intelligence: fixed vs. malleable? • If fixed, less incentive to develop strategies for health promotion • If malleable, more likely to be active in their health care • Studies designed to examine how children think about health and how this affects their behaviors

  23. Scale development • Potential statements tested with 500 healthy high school students • Goal to identify cohesive set of items to categorize beliefs • Entity theorists=health is fixed, unchangeable • Incremental theorists=health is malleable, subject to change • Key items • Your body has a certain amount of health, and you really can’t do much to change it. • Your health is something about you that you can’t change very much. • You can try to make yourself feel better, but you can’t really change your basic health.

  24. Study 1: Test scale with healthy kids • 100 high school students • Exclude any with chronic medical conditions • Asked to respond to scenarios of kids with specific illnesses • Broken leg • Asthma • Appendicitis

  25. Results 1 • Definition of health • Entity attribute to genes • Incremental attribute to behavior • Assessment of health in others • Entity exaggerate illness severity • Entity exaggerate illness duration • Assessment of own health • Incremental theorists believe they are healthier • Entity have higher BMI • Minimal overlap with previous scale of health causation (Locus of control)

  26. Study 2 • 250 high school students • Asked to assess health of others (replicate first study) • Also asked to self-report safety behaviors • Hypothesis: Entity theorists might be less mindful of safety • Results confirmed Study 1 • No significant difference in safety precautions

  27. Current study: Assessment of beliefs in patients • Chronically ill adolescents: Type 1 Diabetes • Age 12-18 yrs • Implicit theories of health • Dependent measures • Self-report of adherence attitudes • Health outcomes • Meter results • Blood sugar levels

  28. Current study 2 • Participants approached at regularly-scheduled clinic visit • Consent from parents • Assent from patients • Participation rate 95% • Questionnaire administered in waiting area • Implicit theory of health scale • Adherence attitudes

  29. Results-adherence attitudes • Entity theorists vs incremental theorists • Less likely to believe that adherence matters (p<.05) • Taking the right amount of medication on schedule will help me control my illness. • If I do exactly what my doctor tells me, I can control my illness.

  30. Results-actual health • Meter results (previous 3 weeks) • Entity vs. incremental • Highest glucose (p<.05) • Mean glucose (p<.05) • Percent above target (p<.02) • Hgb A1c not significant

  31. Overall results • By measuring adolescents’ implicit theories of health, we were able to predict • Reactions to illness in others • Attitudes toward adherence • Actual health outcomes in diabetic patients • Advantages of implicit theory scale (Mueller, Williams, Dweck) • Simple • Easy to administer • Long history of use in various contexts

  32. Future investigations • Apply theory framework to other disease populations • Post-transplant, Cystic fibrosis, IBD • Post-op recovery cardiac surgery • Design experimental manipulations in which theories are changed (eg, fixed to malleable) to measure effect on outcomes • Deliver feedback messages targeted at theory change • Via web-based designs, technologies

  33. Other ongoing research projects • Implicit theories of body weight (Burnette) • Plan to implement interventions (on-line reading activities that present body weight as malleable) • Measure motivation as well as weight loss over test period • Theories of trauma • Teenagers hospitalized for trauma asked about cause of injury • Use attributions to develop prevention strategies • Children’s perceptions of their surgical scars

  34. Summary • Perceptions can change behaviors • In academic setting, children’s beliefs about intelligence influence their performance • In healthcare setting, children’s beliefs about health and illness may affect their responses to disease • My work is an attempt to create a psychological model that explains children’s health behaviors • Encouraging results using implicit theory framework to predict adherence and outcomes in medical setting

  35. Thank you • Carol Dweck, PhD • Robert Wright, MA • Ryan Williams, MD • Bruce Buckingham, MD • Marily Oppezzo, MA • David Yeager, PhD • Jeni Burnette, PhD • Lindsey Eliopulos, MA • MetteHoybye, PhD

  36. References • Berg et al. (1993). Medication compliance: A health care problem. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 27, 2-21. • Burnette, J. (2009). Implicit theories of body weight: Fostering beliefs for healthy behavior. NIH grant proposal. • Cimpian,A, Arce, HC, Markman, EM & Dweck, CS. (2007). Subtle linguistic cues affect children’s motivation. Psychological Science, 18, 314-316. • Diener, CI & Dweck, CS. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 451-462. • Dweck, CS, Chiu, CY & Hong, YY. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267-285. • Festa et al. (1992). Therapeutic adherence to oral medication regimens by adolescents with cancer. J of Pediatrics, 120, 807-811. • Heyman, GD, Dweck, CS & Cain, K. (1992). Young children’s vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness. Child Development, 63, 401-415. • Mueller, CM & Dweck, CS. (1998). Intelligence praise can undermine motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33-52. • Shagena, MM, Sandler, HK & Perrin, EC. (1988). Concepts of illness and perception of control in healthy children and in children with chronic illness. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, 252-256. • Tebbi et al. (1986). Compliance of pediatric and adolescent cancer patients. Cancer, 58, 1179-1184. • Zora et al. (1989). Assessment of compliance in children using inhaled beta adrenergic agonists. Annals of Allergy, 62, 406-409.

More Related