530 likes | 652 Vues
CH 2 CH 2 NH 2. HO. OH. The Playful Mind Play Behavior and the Neurobiology of Having Fun. References to “fun” in the scientific literature are few and far between. Is play fun?. Ask any kid Rats will run a maze when the opportunity to play is the reward
E N D
CH2CH2NH2 HO OH The Playful Mind Play Behavior and the Neurobiology of Having Fun
References to “fun” in the scientific literature are few and far between
Is play fun? • Ask any kid • Rats will run a maze when the opportunity to play is the reward • Rats will prefer an environment where they’ve played over an environment where they haven’t played
The questions of the day • What exactly is play and how do we study it in a rat? • Is there a neural circuit for play? • Since dopamine is involved in everything else, is it also involved in play? • Are there genes for playfulness? • Can studying play in rats tell us anything about psychiatric conditions in human children? • Is this the “fountain of youth”?
What exactly is play and how do we study it in rats?
What exactly is play? Play is repeated, incompletely functional behavior differing from more adaptive versions structurally, contextually, or ontogenetically, and initiated voluntarily when the animal is in a relaxed or unstressed state. Burghardt (2001)
The many faces of play • Imaginative play • Symbolic play • Sensorimotor play • Object play • Rough-and-tumble play
Dependent Measures for Studying Play in the Rat • Contacts directed to nape • Responses to nape contacts • Responses which tend to continue the play bout • Responses which either stop the bout or put the bout “on hold” • Frequency of pins
Response to nape contact Contact directed to nape Complete rotation (pin)
Is there a neural circuit for play? • Experimental approaches • Lesions • Metabolic markers • Administration of neurochemicals
What do we know? • Cerebral cortex not that important • Multiple subcortical circuits • Thalamic / Somatosensory circuit • Basal ganglia circuit • Limbic circuit
Thalamic – somatosensory circuitry • Responsiveness to playful solicitation • Specificity to stimulus type • “tickling” and “laughter”
Thalamic – somatosensory circuitry • Responsiveness to playful solicitation • Specificity to stimulus type • “tickling” and “laughter”
Basal ganglia circuitry • Motor patterning • Motivational component • “sensitized” by lack of stimulation (boredom)
Basal ganglia circuitry • Motor patterning • Motivational component • “sensitized” by lack of stimulation (boredom) Primary motor pathway
Limbic circuitry • Evolution of mammals and the emergence of limbic system • Importance of the social bond
Limbic circuitry • Evolution of mammals and the emergence of limbic system • Importance of the social bond
Is there a neural circuit for play? • Multiple circuits • Cortical development may inhibit subcortical “play circuitry”
Since dopamine seems to be involved in everything else, is it also involved in play? CH2CH2NH2 HO OH
“Dopamine, how do I love thee? Let me count the ways”, jokes George Koob, Ph.D., from the Scripps Institute. Excitement about dopamine is now so high that the danger is not underestimating its reach, but exaggerating it: “Today’s gig is that dopamine is a kind of everyman’s neurotransmitter because it does everything. And the fact is, it doesn’t. “The Plunge of Pleasure” Psychology Today September/October 1997
Brain Dopamine Systems PFC CPu NAc SN VTA Nigrostriatal system Mesolimbic/Mesocortical system
“Tweeking” receptors with neurochemicals • Agonists • Antagonists • Reuptake inhibitors • Reverse-reuptake (release) Administer drug Observe behavior
Dopamine and play • Psychomotor stimulants (amphetamine, methyphenidate) potently reduce play • D1 dopamine agonists and antagonists uniformly reduce play • D2 dopamine agonists reduce play • Low doses may increase play • D2 dopamine antagonists uniformly reduce play • Extensive dopamine lesions alter the patterning of play
Breaking up play Anticipatory/Preparatory Behaviors Consummatory Behaviors
Anticipatory/Preparatory Behaviors Stimuli predictive of reinforcer Increased release of dopamine in mesolimbic terminal regions Increased anticipatory or preparatory responding
Anticipatory Response for Play 5 minutes 5 minutes Control alone alone play partner Play alone Dependent measure: tunnel crosses
Play experience yields an anticipatory response
Blockade of dopamine receptors with haloperidol disrupts anticipatory activity
Anticipatory Behavior and Play Stimuli predictive of play Increased release of dopamine in mesolimbic terminal regions Anticipatory eagerness
Ultrasonic vocalizations as measure of anticipatory eagerness
Acquisition of 55 kHz vocalizations • Control animals placed in chamber for 2 minutes • Experimental animals placed in chamber for 2 minutes prior to a 5 minute opportunity to play
Effects of haloperidol on play • Nape contacts reduced in rats allowed to play with same partner every day • No effects on responsiveness to nape contacts
Since dopamine seems to be involved in everything else, is it also involved in play? • Important for actual execution of the behavior patterns • Anticipatory eagerness • Mesolimbic vs. nigrostriatal involvement CH2CH2NH2 HO OH
Are there genes for playfulness?
Use of inbred strains • Known behavioral and neurobiological differences between selected strains • Fischer-344 and Lewis strains • Groundwork for studying genetics of play • Increased understanding of disorders with genetic origins
Fischer-344 and Lewis strains • Responsiveness to stress • Fischer > Lewis • Reactivity to handling and novelty • Fischer > Lewis • Susceptibility to inflammatory disease • Lewis > Fischer • Self-administration of abused drugs • Lewis > Fischer • Play behavior • Lewis > Fischer
Play solicitation Playful responsiveness
Does isolation affect sensitivity to amphetamine? 60 minutes baseline Social vs. 1 day isolation Fischer or Lewis Amphetamine (2 mg/kg) Social vs. 3 days Isolation 90 minutes post-injection
Amphetamine has comparable effect after 1 day of isolation
3 days of isolation results in sensitized response in Lewis rats, but not Fischer rats
Is there a parallel with drug use and abuse? Isolation-induced sensitization Chronic, intermittent bouts of play isolation Sensitized response to amphetamine Drug-induced sensitization Chronic, intermittent drug taking withdrawal Sensitized response to amphetamine
Are there genes for playfulness? • Strain differences • Independence of maternal influence • Individual differences? • Personality traits?
Can studying play in rats tell us anything about psychiatric conditions in human children? • Attention deficit disorder (ADHD) • Autism • Childhood depression • Anti-social behaviors and violent tendencies
Is this the “fountain of youth”
Why is it important for your child to play? When children play, they exercise their senses, their intellect, their emotions, their imagination - keenly and energetically…to play is to explore, to discover and to experiment. Playing helps children develop ideas and gain experience. It gives them a wealth of knowledge and information about the world in which they live - and about themselves. So to play is also to learn. Play is fun for children. But it’s much more than that - it’s good for them, and it’s necessary…play gives children the opportunity to develop and use the many talents they were born with.