1 / 58

Design Issues: Policy Trials

Design Issues: Policy Trials. Professor David Torgerson Director, York Trials Unit djt6@york.ac.uk. Policy Trials. These are similar to ordinary RCTs. We need to undertaken trials that are unbiased and cost effective. Avoiding selection bias at randomisation; Cluster trial design;

Télécharger la présentation

Design Issues: Policy Trials

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Design Issues: Policy Trials Professor David Torgerson Director, York Trials Unit djt6@york.ac.uk

  2. Policy Trials • These are similar to ordinary RCTs. We need to undertaken trials that are unbiased and cost effective. • Avoiding selection bias at randomisation; • Cluster trial design; • Efficient design.

  3. Selection Bias • Selection bias can occur in non-randomised studies when group selection is related to a known or unknown prognostic variable. • If the variable is either unknown or imperfectly measured then it is not possible to control for this confound and the observed effect may be biased.

  4. Effects of selection bias • Observational data on hormone replacement therapy consistently shows that this reduces cardiovascular disease, stroke, dementia. • Trial evidence shows the opposite. • BECAUSE women taking HRT tended to be different and at lower risk from these problems than women not taking HRT.

  5. Randomisation • Randomisation (or a similar technique, such as minimisation) removes selection bias across a ‘population’ of RCTs by ensuring all variables that may affect outcome are balanced across treatment groups at baseline. • Other techniques may allow the introduction of selection bias.

  6. Subversion • Subversion of the allocation mechanism introduces selection bias. • This occurs when the next allocation can be predicted and participants are then selected to match a desired allocation rather than having the allocation assigned at random.

  7. Subversion - evidence • Schulz [1] has described incidents of researchers subverting allocation by looking at sealed envelopes through x-ray lights. Researchers have confessed to breaking open filing cabinets to obtain the randomisation code. • In a survey [2] of 25 researchers 4 admitted to keeping ‘a log’ of previous allocations to try and predict future allocations. • Case study of a subverted trial. Schulz JAMA 1995;274:1456. Brown et al. Stats in Medicine, 2005,24:3715.

  8. Mean ages of groups

  9. Example of Subversion

  10. Recent Blocked Trial “This was a block randomised study (four patientsto each block) with separate randomisation at each of the threecentres. Blocks of four cards were produced, each containing twocards marked with "nurse" and two marked with "house officer."Each card was placed into an opaque envelope and the envelopesealed. The block was shuffled and, after shuffling, was placedin a box.” Kinley et al., BMJ 325:1323.

  11. What is wrong here? Kinley et al., BMJ 325:1323.

  12. Problem? • If block randomisation of 4 were used then each centre should not be different by more than 2 patients in terms of group sizes. • Two centres had a numerical disparity of 11. Either blocks of 4 were not used or the sequence was not followed.

  13. Evidence from a systematic review. • In a systematic review of the use of calcium supplements to enhance weight loss Trowman et al found a significant relationship between calcium use and reductions in body weight. • HOWEVER, examination of baseline characteristics found that people with lower body weights tended to be allocated to the calcium group. In no single trial was this difference significant but in a meta-analysis of baseline weights the difference was highly significant (p = 0.006).

  14. Meta-analysis of baseline body weight. Trowman et al. The impact of baseline imbalances should be considered in systematic reviews: a methodological case study. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2007;60:1229-1233

  15. Why subversion? • “To provide best patient care…” • “He fancied her! She was pretty!” • “Individual was putting younger fitter individuals into the intervention, they were trying to improve the results” • “Prefer to do certain procedures” • “Researcher over rode the random allocation, thought there should be same numbers in each group” Hewitt et al. J Clin Epidemiol 2009;62:261-69

  16. Concealment: Recommendations • Allocation sequence must be independently generated and kept secret from the people who are enrolling participants. • A secure method of giving allocation to the recruiters must be developed, coin tossing, or opaque envelopes are inadequate.

  17. Cluster trials • In most drug RCTs people are randomised as individuals to treatment. However, many policy trials need to randomise intact groups (e.g., schools, prisons, hospitals; periods of time). • These trials can have problems because often difficult to conceal allocation of cluster from person recruiting participants.

  18. Recruitment Bias • A key issue is individual participant recruitment into cluster trials. • There are a number of ways where biased participant recruitment can occur, which can lead to baseline imbalances in important prognostic factors. • In an individually randomised trial we avoid recruitment bias by concealing the random allocation from the potential participant and researcher until AFTER they have consented to be in the trial and have been recruited. • In cluster trials sometimes this is not possible.

  19. Identification Problems • For example, in a cluster trial of back pain treatments equal number of patients with same severity of back pain will be present in both clusters. The problem lies in how to identify such patients to include them in the interventions. Unless one is very careful different numbers and types of patient can be selected.

  20. UK BEAM Trial • The UKBEAM pilot study used a cluster design. Eligible patients were identified by GPs for trial inclusion. • GP practices were randomised to usual care or extra training. • The ‘primary care team’ were trained to deliver ‘active’ management of backpain.

  21. UK BEAM participant recruitment P = 0.06 P = 0.01 P = 0.01

  22. UKBEAM pilot study.

  23. Another musculoskeletal trial • In 2002 I joined a steering group for a trial of training GPs to identify and treat a common musculoskeletal condition. • GPs were to recruit the participants. • With the BEAM experience we KNOW what WILL happen. • GPs WILL recruit more patients if they are trained. • Did they?

  24. Why would you do that? • “You learn nothing by being kicked by the same mule twice”.

  25. Consent Bias • This occurs when consent to take part in the trial occurs AFTER randomisation. • Another danger in Cluster trials. • For example, Graham et al, randomised schools to a teaching package for emergency contraception. More children took part in the intervention than the control. Graham et al. BMJ 2002;324:1179.

  26. Consent bias? Knowledge of emergency contraception at baseline

  27. Consent Bias? • Because more children consented in the intervention group we would expect their knowledge to be less (as we include children less likely to know). • Conversely we get a volunteer or consent effect with the intervention group only those most knowledgeable agreeing to take part.

  28. Trial Consent Problems • Even when it is possible to identify all eligible members of a cluster some may not consent to take part in the trial. If there is differential consent, in particular, this can lead to selection bias again. • To prevent this we must use the same approach as we do for individually randomised trials: recruit participants on the basis that the can get either intervention and then randomise.

  29. Hip Protector Trial At this point trial is balanced for all co-variates Kannus. N Eng J Med 2000;343:1506.

  30. Hip Protector Trial Selection Bias

  31. Dilution effects • In a cluster trial of accident prevention among young children 25% of parents in the experimental arm did not receive the intervention. Clearly this will reduce the power of that trial AND dilute any likely ‘treatment’ effect. Kendrick et al. BMJ 1999;318:980.

  32. Review of Cluster Trials • Because of the ‘BEAM’ problem we decided to undertake a methodological review of cluster trials. • We identified all cluster trials published in the BMJ, Lancet, NEJM since 1997. Puffer et al. BMJ 2003;327:785.

  33. Results • We identified 36 relevant trials. ONLY 13 had identified participants prior to randomisation. • Of the 23 not identifying participants a priori 7 showed evidence of differential recruitment or consent. • Other biases included differential of inclusion criteria or attrition. • In total 14 (39%) showed evidence of bias.

  34. Underestimate of problem • Only in 5 papers did authors alert reader to possible problem. • Subsequently one of the trials that ‘looked’ OK was published elsewhere where recruitment bias was admitted to have occurred.

  35. Baseline Characteristics Jordhoy Palliative Medicine 2002 16:43-49.

  36. Cluster Trials: Should I do one? • Yes, BUT do them properly. • Is it possible to avoid doing them and do an individually randomised trial?

  37. Contamination • An important justification for their use is SUPPOSED ‘contamination’ between participants allocated to the intervention with people allocated to the control.

  38. Spurious Contamination? • Trial proposal to cluster randomise practices for a breast feeding study – new mothers might talk to each other! • Trial for reducing cardiac risk factors patients again might talk to each other. • Trial for removing allergens from homes of asthmatic children.

  39. Patient level contamination • In a trial of counselling adults to reduce their risk of cardiovascular disease general practices were randomised to avoid contamination of control participants by intervention patients. Steptoe. BMJ 1999;319:943.

  40. Counselling Trial • Steptoe et al, wanted to detect a 9% reduction in smoking prevalence with a health promotion intervention. They needed 2000 participants (rather than 1282) because of clustering. • If they had randomised 2000 individuals this would have been able to detect a 7% reduction allowing for a 20% CONTAMINATION. Steptoe. BMJ 1999;319:943.

  41. Accepting Contamination • We should accept some contamination and deal with it through individual randomisation and by boosting the sample size rather than going for cluster randomisation Torgerson BMJ 2001;322:355.

  42. What about dilution bias? • If, in the presence of contamination, we use individual allocation we might observe a difference that is statistically significant but is not clinically or economically significant. • Dilution has biased the estimate towards the mean. • If we can measure contamination we can deal with this using ‘instrumental’ or CACE analysis. Hewitt et al. Canadian Medical Association Journal 2006;175:347-48

  43. Cluster Trials • Can cluster trials give different results? • All things being equal this shouldn’t happen (except for a more imprecise estimate). BUT because of the greater potential for selection bias cluster trials MAY give the ‘wrong’ answer.

  44. An example. • There are 14 RCTs of hip protectors for the prevention of hip fracture. • Nine RCTs are individually randomised trials, whilst 5 are cluster trials (e.g., hospital ward, nursing home). • Cluster trials, without exception show a benefit of hip protectors.

  45. Hip Protector Trials

  46. Hip Protector Trials: Cluster vs Individually Randomised.

  47. Age differences between ‘good’ cluster and poor cluster trials. Data from Puffer et al.

More Related