1 / 32

SEL2211: Contexts Lecture 18: Artificial Intelligence

This lecture provides an overview of the history, aims, and future prospects of Artificial Intelligence (AI), a discipline focused on designing intelligent machines. It explores the concept of intelligence and the challenges in defining it. The lecture also discusses the possibility of replacing "intelligence" with "cognition" in the field's title and delves into the rich history of AI research.

haroldw
Télécharger la présentation

SEL2211: Contexts Lecture 18: Artificial Intelligence

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence In a now-famous research paper published in 1950 called Computing Machinery and Intelligence, the inventor of the digital electronic computer, Alan Turing, asked whether a suitably programmed computer could think like a human. An academic discipline, Artificial Intelligence (AI), came into being to answer that question. This lecture outlines the history of  AI, its current state, and future prospects. It is best read after reviewing Lecture 8: Language and Computation. The discussion is in three main parts: The aim of AI research History of AI research Issues in AI

  2. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research As its name indicates, the aim of AI is to design and physically implement machines whose intelligence is indistinguishable from that of biological humans. When setting out to do something, it's advisable to be clear about what that something is. AI sets out to design and implement intelligent machines, so it's reasonable at the outset to be clear about what intelligence is.

  3. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research We all have commonsense intuitions about intelligence based on experience --that most people are roughly equally intelligent, that some people are more intelligent than most, and that some are less intelligent or even stupid. When asked to say what we mean by 'intelligence', though, we generally flounder; there's definitely something called 'intelligence' and some people have more of it than others, but we can't say precisely what it is.

  4. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research After much philosophical and psychological debate over many centuries, moreover, there's no agreed definition of 'intelligence'. In 1994 a group of 52 academics involved in intelligence-related research published a statement on what they considered it to be, and it is quoted here both because it represents a consensus of more or less current academic opinion on the subject, and because it shows how imprecise that consensus is even among professionals. Intelligence, according to the 52, is ...a very general mental capability that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly and learn from experience. It is not merely book learning, a narrow academic skill, or test-taking smarts. Rather, it reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings—"catching on," "making sense" of things, or "figuring out" what to do.

  5. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research IQ tests which purport to measure this elusive human characteristic exist, and these are often criticized for endowing an ill-defined concept with a spurious scientific legitimacy. Special-purpose tests focused on specific human abilities such as mathematics or verbal articulacy are better because the things being measured are well-defined, and it turns out that there is a positive correlation between such special-purpose tests and the much-criticized tests of general intelligence, which lends some support to our intuitions about intelligence. In general, though, 'intelligence' remains an ill-defined concept.

  6. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research A less controversial and more productive way of understanding what AI sets out to do is to replace 'intelligence' with 'cognition' in its title, that is, to call the discipline 'Artificial Cognition' (AC). Needless the say, there's little prospect of that happening because 'Artificial Intelligence' is historically well entrenched. Why would that help? 'Cognition' is a general term for the collection of human mental capabilities such as sensory interpretation of the world (by, for example, vision and audition), memory, reasoning, learning, and language.

  7. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 1. The Aim of AI research These aspects of mind are readily definable: we know and can say explicitly what vision is, we know and can say explicitly what language is, and so on. Artificial Intelligence, or rather Artificial Cognition, would then be the discipline whose remit is to design and implement machines which emulate human cognitive capabilities. In practice, that's exactly what AI does.

  8. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research People have always been fascinated by the possibility of creating intelligent machines, as the 'History of Artificial Intelligence Research' link at the end of the lecture shows. In the past this fascination has been articulated in the form of myths, fiction, philosophical speculation, and the construction of automata, that is, mechanical models of creatures which, in a very limited way, emulated their living counterparts. One example is the silver swan at Bowes Museum in Barnard Castle, County Durham, which can be seen at this link.

  9. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research AI research in the modern scientific sense begins with Alan Turing's invention of the computer and his proposal for a program of research into whether or not this type of computer might be programmed to emulate human intelligence. This proposal set the agenda for the early development of AI. Specifically: The artificially intelligent machines that AI aimed to design and implement would be Turing computers, and The design for an artificially intelligent Turing computer would be an algorithm, that is, a computer program.

  10. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research AI became a scientific discipline at a conference held at Dartmouth College, USA, in 1956, and many of those attending, such as Marvin Minsky and John McCarthy, became important figures in the field. The possibilities discussed there caused great excitement, both in the scientific community and, not long thereafter, in the general public, which was gradually becoming aware of what at the time were thought of as 'thinking machines', that is, computers. Ground-breaking research was done in areas such as reasoning and natural language, and as work progressed optimistic claims were made for AI.

  11. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research Wikipedia has collected a few examples of such claims made by prominent scientists in the field: 1958, H. A. Simon and Allen Newell: "Within ten years a digital computer will be the world's chess champion" and "Within ten years a digital computer will discover and prove an important new mathematical theorem.“ 1965, H. A. Simon: "Machines will be capable, within twenty years, of doing any work a man can do.“ 1967, Marvin Minsky: "Within a generation ... the problem of creating 'artificial intelligence' will substantially be solved.“ 1970, Marvin Minsky: "In from three to eight years we will have a machine with the general intelligence of an average human being."

  12. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research The popular media featured speculations on topics such as whether switching off an artificially intelligent computer would be murder, and robots with human-level cognitive abilities became the staple of the science fiction literary genre and of movies and television; a few of the better films are:

  13. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research Alas, it was not to be. Despite substantial governmental and commercial research funding, progress was slow and ambitious projects failed to live up to expectations. By the end of the 1970s the nature of the problem of emulating human cognitive capabilities had become much clearer, and most AI researchers had realized that initial expectations had been hopelessly over-optimistic. Emulating human cognitive capability had turned out to be a very difficult problem indeed. Because of poor existing results and daunting prospects for future success, research funding was scaled back, and, from about 1980, the AI community looked for alternative ways to proceed.

  14. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research The second phase of AI research since about 1980 is characterized by the following developments:

  15. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research i. The field has fragmented. AI began as a relatively small, coherent research community focused on a specific goal, and for two decades it remained pretty much that way. Post-c.1980 there has been increasing specialization in the sense that researchers now tend to work on specific cognitive functions such as vision, speech, and language. There have been few if any recent attempts to build machines with general human-level cognition. The aim, rather, is to better understand individual cognitive functions and to design and implement computational systems which emulate them, leaving integration into a single system as a future goal.

  16. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research ii. There has been an increasing emphasis on learning as an alternative to explicit design. In the first phase of AI the emphasis was on explicit design of cognitive algorithms: the designer specified the algorithms and the data structures that they manipulated and implemented them on computers by writing computer programs. Some researchers felt that the lack of success using this approach was due to the difficultly of the problem --that understanding cognitive functions and designing algorithms to implement them computationally was too difficult for humans to solve explicitly. Instead, they began to develop algorithms which would allow computers to learn human cognitive behaviour by interaction with the real-world environment. The inspiration for this came from two directions.

  17. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research Neuroscience The brain is the organism in the body that implements our cognitive functions: sensory processing, reasoning, memory, language, and that sense of self-awareness which we call consciousness. By analogy with a computer, if cognition is human software, then the brain is its hardware. An earlier lecture described the structure of the brain and how that structure changes in response to sensory input via learning. Taking brain structure and the brain's learning mechanism as a guide, AI researchers have developed neurally-inspired computational models called artificial neural networks (ANN) that learn cognitive functions from examples presented to them as input.

  18. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research Neuroscience A famous example is the past-tense learning model. A series of (present tense / past tense) pairs like (jump / jumped), (kick / kicked) and so on was presented to an ANN; after learning a sufficient number of examples, the network correctly supplied the correct past tense to present tense forms it had not previously seen. In other words, it had learned a small part of the cognitive function of language without needing to be explicitly programmed. This idea has been extended to other aspects of language as well as to other cognitive functions.

  19. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 2. History of AI research Embodied cognition Until the late 1980s cognitive science had been concerned pretty much exclusively with the structure and mechanisms of the mind, as one would expect. By the late 1980s, however, it was increasingly argued that, to understand cognition, one has to take account of human interaction with the real-world environment to which the human responds, in which he or she acts, and from which he or she learns. This innovation is known as 'situated cognition' because it studies cognition within its real-world context or situation, or 'embodied cognition' because it studies cognition as the control mechanism for the body's interaction with the environment.

  20. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Many important issues have still to be resolved before AI can have a realistic hope of achieving its goal, some theoretical and some technical, and there is no way we can review all or even most of these here. This section therefore focuses on what is regarded by many if not most AI researchers as the most fundamental issue of all: meaning.

  21. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Turing's 1950 paper Computing Machinery and Intelligence opens with the words: 'I propose to consider the question, "Can machines think?" '. He recognized that 'think' is an ill-defined word, like 'intelligence', and that any argument for artificial intelligence based on it would generate interminable discussion about what thinking is, so he proposed instead  an 'imitation game'. This game would be set in two rooms A and B separated by a wall without windows or doors. In the room A would be a human, and in B would be either a computer or another human; the human in room A would know it was one or the other, but not which at any given time. The human in room A can communicate with the computer / human in B by writing and receiving messages on a computer just as we communicate using email or instant messaging today.

  22. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI The game consists of the human in A having a series of text-based conversations with the computer / human in B; sometimes this would be with the computer and sometimes with a human, but the human in A would not be told which. If the human in A cannot reliably tell when s/he is conversing with the computer or with a human in B, then, in Turing's words 'If a machine acts as intelligently as a human being, then it is as intelligent as a human being'.

  23. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI The essential question that the above Turing Test raises is this: Is a computational simulation of human intelligence equivalent to biological human intelligence? That question has generated extensive philosophical discussion; for an overview see the 'Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence' link at the end of the lecture. The discussion  is sometimes enlightening, sometimes confusing, and occasionally confused, but a highly influential answer has been and continues to be that given by John Searle, who maintains that the computational simulation of intelligence is definitely not equivalent to biological human intelligence. Searle's argument is based on the Chinese Room thought experiment, which has already been discussed in an earlier lecture.

  24. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Imagine an auditorium full of Chinese speakers, and on the stage a box the size of a typical room in a house. The box has only two openings on opposite walls. One opening is a slot into which slips of paper containing Mandarin Chinese text may be inserted, and the other is a slot from which  slips of paper containing the Cantonese Chinese translation of the input slips emerges. In other words, the box is a Mandarin-to-Cantonese translation device. Members of the audience are invited to input slips in Mandarin and, when the output slip emerges, to judge whether or not the translation is accurate.

  25. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Let's say that, after many trials, the audience concludes that the translations have been correct in every case. One could then say that the device has simulated a human cognitive function --the ability to translate from one natural language dialect to another. The room is clearly a version of the Turing Test.

  26. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI The question Searle asks, though, is whether the translation device is intelligent in the same sense that a human translator is intelligent. To answer this, he allows us to look inside the box. It contains the necessary paper slips and a pen to produce the output slips, a shelf on which sit a Mandarin-Cantonese dictionary and some books on Chinese grammar, and John Searle himself. When a slip arrives at the input slot, Searle takes it, and using the grammar books and dictionary, writes the translation onto a slip and outputs it.

  27. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI But here is the important point: Searle knows no Chinese. All he does is follow rules for transforming one string of symbols into another string of symbols. From this he concludes that a simulation of a human cognitive function is not equivalent to the human cognitive function it is simulating, and that the device is consequently not intelligent in the same sense that a human translator is intelligent. Why?

  28. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI The room is clearly a Turing Machine, a computer that moves physical symbols around according to an algorithm which allows it to simulate a human cognitive function. But there is a crucial difference between the computer and the members of the audience: the humans understand natural language symbol strings in the sense that they attribute meaning to them, whereas the computer does not. Moreover, given that computers as Turing defined them have no capacity for including meaning in their design, they can never be intelligent in the sense that a human is intelligent.

  29. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Searle's Chinese Room has generated a huge amount of discussion, which is summarized by the 'Chinese Room' link at the end of the lecture. The debate goes on, much of it concentrating on what exactly the elusive human capacity for meaning might be, whether or not it is possible in principle to incorporate it into the capability of computers, and, if so, how to do it. One approach, described here, is to address the problem of symbol grounding introduced in Lecture 6: The Computational Theory of Mind.

  30. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Recall that symbol grounding with reference to language involves associating linguistic symbols, that is, words and word strings, with bodily experience of the world; the association is what gives the linguistic symbols their meaning. At a simple level, the word 'cat' means the sum total of one's individual experience of cats in the real world, and 'love' the emotional experience of closeness to another human. How can such experience of the world be incorporated into computational AI systems?

  31. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Issues in AI Section 2 above has already given the answer: provide sensory input mechanisms as part of the computational architecture of AI systems, and replace the Turing Machine architecture of traditional AI systems with an artificial neural network architecture so that the system can learn from its sensory input and thereby acquire the association between symbols and their referents, that is, meaning. This symbol grounding approach to AI is a complex topic, and is further developed in one of the seminars associated with this lecture.

  32. SEL2211: ContextsLecture 18: Artificial Intelligence 3. Where next? Emergent behaviour in complex nonlinear systems: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=gdQgoNitl1g http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=S5NRNG1r_jI http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Lk6QU94xAb8

More Related