1 / 15

An Intuitive Explanation of Bayes ' Theorem

An Intuitive Explanation of Bayes ' Theorem. By Eliezer Yudkowsky. reference. http://yudkowsky.net/rational/bayes. Questions.

keahi
Télécharger la présentation

An Intuitive Explanation of Bayes ' Theorem

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. An Intuitive Explanation of Bayes' Theorem By EliezerYudkowsky

  2. reference • http://yudkowsky.net/rational/bayes

  3. Questions • 100 out of 10,000 women at age forty who participate in routine screening have breast cancer.  80 of every 100 women with breast cancer will get a positive mammography.  950 out of  9,900 women without breast cancer will also get a positive mammography.  If 10,000 women in this age group undergo a routine screening, about what fraction of women with positive mammographies will actually have breast cancer?

  4. Compute • A= positive mammographies & actually have breast cancer • B = positive mammographies • results = A/B *100% • A=100*(80/100)=80 • B= women with breast cancer with a positive mammography (A) + women without  breast cancer with a positive mammography (C) • B= 100 * (80/100) +(10000-100) *(950/9900) • results = A/B *100% =80/1030=7.8%

  5. Questions – different version • 1% of women at age forty who participate in routine screening have breast cancer.  80% of women with breast cancer will get positive mammographies.  9.6% of women without breast cancer will also get positive mammographies.  A woman in this age group had a positive mammography in a routine screening.  What is the probability that she actually has breast cancer? • A=1%*80% • B=A+(1-1%)*9.6% • Results=A/B*100%=7.8%

  6. Egg problem • Some eggs are painted red and some are painted blue.  40% of the eggs in the bin contain pearls, and 60% contain nothing.   30% of eggs containing pearls are painted blue, and 10% of eggs containing nothing are painted blue.  What is the probability that a blue egg contains a pearl? • p(pearl) = 40% • p(blue|pearl) = 30% • p(blue|~pearl) = 10% • p(pearl|blue) = ? • "~" is shorthand for "not", so ~pearl reads "not pearl". • blue|pearl is shorthand for "blue given pearl“ • "the probability that an egg is painted blue, given that the egg contains a pearl".   • the order of implication is read right-to-left • blue|pearl means "blue<-pearl", the degree to which pearl-ness implies blue-ness • <d|c><c|b><b|a> reads as "the probability that a particle at A goes to B, then to C, ending up at D". 

  7. Visualized Results

  8. More notation • The item on the right side = what you already know or the premise, • The item on the left side = the implication or conclusion. •  p(blue|pearl) = 30%, • we already know that some egg contains a pearl, then we can conclude there is a 30% chance that the egg is painted blue.  • p(pearl|blue) • "the chance that a blue egg contains a pearl" or • "the probability that an egg contains a pearl, if we know the egg is painted blue"   • p(pearl|blue) = p(pear&blue) / p(blue)

  9. Bayes' Theorem • A=1%*80% • B=A+(1-1%)*9.6% • Results=A/B*100%=7.8% • A=p(cancer)*p(positive|cancer) • B=A+ p(~cancer) *p(positive|~cancer) • A/B= p(cancer|positive)

  10. Bayes' Theorem • What we know • P(A)=15% • P(E| A =10%) • What we also know • P(~A)=1- 15% =85% • P(E|~ A) =80% • Why not 1- 10% ? • What we want to know • Probability of area ? in given E • P(A|E)=? • How? A ? E p(E | A )p(A ) = p(A | E) p(E) p(E | A )p(A ) = + p(E | ~A )p(~A ) p(E | A )p(A )

  11. Bayes' Theorem A2 A3 A4 A1 E A6 A5 p(E | A )p(A ) p(E | A )p(A ) = = i i i i p(A | E) å i p(E) p(E | A )p(A ) j j j where {Ai} forms a partition of the event space, • Based on definition of conditional probability • p(Ai|E) is posterior probability given evidence E • p(Ai) is the prior probability • P(E|Ai) is the likelihood of the evidence given Ai • p(E) is the preposterior probability of the evidence

  12. likelihood*prior Posterior= evidence p(E | A )p(A ) = i i p(A | E) i p(E)

  13. Example • You go to the doctor’s office, where you take a test for a horrible disease • The test is 99% accurate • If the test is positive, 99% of the time if you have the disease, negative 99% if you don’t • The disease itself is rare: occurs in I in 10,000 people • Your test is positive. What is the probability you have the disease?

  14. Why is this useful? • Useful for assessing diagnostic probability from causal probability • P(cause|effect)= P(effect|cause)P(cause) P(effect) • Let M be meningitis, S be stiff neckP(m|s)=P(s|m)P(m) = 0.8 X 0.0001 = 0.0008 P(s) 0.1 • Note: posterior probability of meningitis is still very small!

  15. Homework • Write a simulation for Monty Hall problem • Suppose you're on a game show, and you're given the choice of three doors: Behind one door is a car; behind the others, goats. You pick a door, say No. 1, and the host, who knows what's behind the doors, opens another door, say No. 3, which has a goat. He then says to you, "Do you want to pick door No. 2?" Is it to your advantage to switch your choice? 

More Related