310 likes | 436 Vues
The Psychological Functions of Function Words Cindy Chung and James Pennebaker. And perhaps also Measurement of Negativity Bias in Personal Narratives Using Corpus-Based Emotion Dictionaries Shuki J. Cohen. Function Words. Content words: nouns and regular verbs
E N D
The Psychological Functions of Function WordsCindy Chung and James Pennebaker And perhaps also Measurement of Negativity Bias in Personal Narratives Using Corpus-Based Emotion Dictionaries Shuki J. Cohen Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Function Words • Content words: nouns and regular verbs • Function words: pronouns, prepositions, articles, conjunctions, and auxiliary verbs. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Fn words factoids • vocabulary of well over 100,000 words, fewer than 400 are function words (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Gulikers, 1995). • Neo-Zipfian factoid: less than 0.04% of our vocabulary accounts for over half of the words we use in daily speech (Rochon, Saffran, Berndt, & Schwartz, 2000). • Despite the frequency of their use, they are the hardest to master when learning a new language (Weber-Fox & Neville, 2001). • We don’t remember the usage of fn words • We have little control over the usage frequency of fn words Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Mind and Brain factoids of fn words • Damage to Wernicke’s area – damage to content words (Miller, 1995). • Damage to Broca’s area – damage to speech speed and fn words • Function words vary according to psychological states and personalities • Allport (1961) emphasized the idea of stylistic behaviors or, more broadly, personality styles. • [authorship attribution: “whilst” in Federalist Papers. (Mosteller and Wallace, 1964) ] Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Methodological issues • Self reports tend to reflect speaker’s theory of self, rather than true self condition; cheap to collect • Analyzing the specific meaning of content words in context; tedious and expensive (manual work). • Bag of words? • Analyzing words is easy with the advancement of computers and electronic text. • problematic; ignores context, idioms • Fn words don’t have that problem • forced looking at word usage, not (only?) broader word meaning Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Effects of what? • How can we say that the various effects that we have discussed reflect function word differences and not differences in content or context? • Perhaps these effects are merely reflections of differences in syntax. • People can choose the content words (where and what to talk about), but less so the fn words? Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Reflect of Affect? • Do function words reflect or influence psychological state? • reflection of cog state? • a Whorfian view? • They failed to make people changethe pronouns they used (from I to We)in order to feel part of a group , and so they take it to be supporting a cog reflection view. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Some more big words • “We are now standing at the gates of a new age of understanding the links between language and personality” Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
LIWC • Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count, or LIWC (Pennebaker, Francis, & Booth, 2001). • human judgments for how 2000 words or word stems were related to each of several dozen categories. • Found connection b/w words used and health states after trauma Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Function of what again? • All function words? • Not really. • Most frequent nn function words? • Not really. • Only Pronouns? • Not really. • Only I! • And a few other pronouns in comparison to I. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Testosterone! • Lower levels – less usage of “I”. • but found no other linguistic correlates to mood etc. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Where does depression hurt? • In your “I”! • use of first person singular is associated with negative affective states (Weintraub, 1989). • currently depressed > formerly depressed > never depressed students. In writing about coming to college (Rude, Gortner, & Pennebaker, 2004). • Also in natural speech (Mehl, 2004). • In both studies, pronouns are a better marker of depression than negative emotion words. • analysis of the poetry of suicidal versus non-suicidal poets Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Individual Stress • Text analyses of Giuliani’s press conferences in the months surrounding his personal upheavals: use of “I”/”me” up from 2% of his words to over 7% (Pennebaker & Lay, 2002). • In Guiliani’s press conferences during his first four years of mayor, he used “we” words at exceptionally high rates – over 2.5%. When his life fell apart, this rate dropped to the normal 1%. • his use of “we” words in his early mayor period was judged as marked by distanced or royal “we” words; but his post-9/11 “we” words referred to specific individuals or identifiable groups. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Socially-shared stress • usage of third person indicates healthy mental state. • Switching from high rates of “I” to high rates of other personal pronouns when writing about emotional upheavals in their lives --> greater health improvements in the months after writing (Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003). • Used LSA. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Sad is not Depressed • relatively healthy people facing the upheavals of 9/11 actually evidenced a drop in “I” words rather than an increase. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Deception • when people have been induced to describe or explain something honestly or deceptively, the combined use of first person singular pronouns and exclusive words predicts honesty (Newman, Pennebaker, Berry, & Richards, 2003). Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Status • In dyads: person whose use of “I” words is lower tends to be the higher status participant. • In the analysis of the incoming and outgoing emails of 11 undergraduates, graduate students, and faculty, the rated status of the interactant was correlated −.40 with the relative use of “I” words (Pennebaker & Davis, 2006). • Duh! • Me: “I did X and Y…” • Advisor: “Why haven’t you done Z??” Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Gender • females use first person singular pronouns more than males. Newman et al. (2003) • females are generally more self-focused than men? • more prone to depression than men? • have traditionally held lower status positions relative to men? • Males use more articles and nouns in natural speech and writing (categorization, concrete thinking?). females use more (especially auxiliary) verbs (relational orientations?) Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Age • people use fewer first person singular words and more first person plural words with age Pennebaker and Stone (2003). • greater use of exclusive words • as people age they make more distinctions and psychologically distance themselves from their topics. • older people speak with greater cog complexity. • use more future tense and less past tense the older they get. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Culture (cross-linguistic) • first person plural pronouns: USA and Japan use them in a close, personal way at the same rates. • American authors used first person plural pronouns in a distant, royal-we way at double the rate that was found in the Japanese texts. • This accounted for the overall greater rate of first person plural pronouns in American than in Japanese texts. • counter to stereotypes, the Japanese texts used first person singular pronouns at a higher rate than did American texts. • American texts were higher in their use of first person plural pronouns (Chung & Pennebaker, 2005). Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
More cultural speculations • overall, “I” use reflects self-focus. • focus on the self is required to achieve collectivistic values such as harmony, empathy, and self-criticism to please the ingroup (e.g. Kanagawa, Cross, & Markus, 2001; Markus & Kitayama, 1991), • use of “we”: feelings of closeness and of sharing a common fate with another, more than “Other and I” (Fitzsimmons & Kay, 2004), “they”, or “it” (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Culture III • western philosophy more on categorization, eastern asian philosophy more on movement and process. • Translations from Japanese contain lesser fn words such as a, an, the, that come before nouns (categories) (Chung & Pennebaker, 2005). • [what about individual findings replicated in other languages?] Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Socially-shared stress II • In community-wide upheaval, use of first person plural pronouns increases: • chat room discussions after Princess Diana’s death (Stone & Pennebaker, 2002); newspaper accounts of the Texas A&M Bonfire tragedy (Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003); over 1000 bloggers who were tracked in the months before and after 9/11 (Cohn, Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2004); Also in speech (Mehl & Pennebaker, 2003). Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Shuki Cohen Measurement of Negativity Bias in Personal Narratives Using Corpus-Based Emotion Dictionaries Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Neurotics and Friends • Theory assumes neurotics is a problem in emotional processing and expresses in languages b/c language also uses emotional info processing. • Neurotics selectively attend to, interpret, and recall neg charged and ambig event • people with negative affectivity, depressive self-schema, dysphoric, Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
theory-driven • Theory assumes pos and neg words are independent. • Checks if correlation exists b/w usage of a-priori (?) suspected words and mental health indices • Unlike Empirical approach: • index all words of neurotics and non-neurotics and find subset that gives best discrimination Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Materials • Usage of (transcribed) speech, not text • Self report questionnaires susceptive to introspective limits, self-deception, defensiveness • Narratives about self or sig. other • Narratives on other persons were not found useful (not correlating mental health states) • Emotional word list dictionaries • filtered by usage (e.g., “like”, pretty” etc. although positive, are mainly used as intensifiers, hence “diluting” the positive meaning). • Used both manually constructed lists from patients’ narratives and existing dictionaries. Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Evaluation • Manual evaluations • Slow, expensive • Subject to rater drift and biases • Automatic evaluations (corpus-based) • Fast • Showed better results in some studies assessing mental health • Interesting for us! Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Emotion lists • Two master lists: Positive list, negative list • Exception list for each • Idioms / ngrams without the same emotional value: “alright” is positive, but “it was alright, I guess” – quite neurtal • Negations - up to 3 tokens apart • cannot handle double neg, as we mentioned last time • claims not to find statistically enough cases of double neg in pilot study. • “like” is filtered from positive list, but “didn’t like” is useful in neg list! Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Results • Found higher correlations / effects than Pennebaker • Mental health indices are highly correlated with usage of negativity in speech • Pennebaker found sig corr. with indices of mental health, but very small effects • Here the magnitude of the effects is better, and more correlated Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group
Fine Correlations • Not a single count: not subtracting neg counts from pos counts. • Positive Correlation between psychological profile (neuroticism level by questionnaires) and counts of negative normalized by total tokens (not pos-neg), • negative correlation with counts of pos words. • But not necessarily neg correlation b/w pos and neg words! Yuval Marton, Sentiment Analysis Reading Group