1 / 43

Beyond SatNav: Using the Global Positioning System as a tool for the behavioural sciences

gretel
Télécharger la présentation

Beyond SatNav: Using the Global Positioning System as a tool for the behavioural sciences

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


    2. Collaborators Iain Gilchrist Bruce Hood Kirsten Cater Niall Alcock Kylie Nott Naimah Ikram

    3. Global Positioning System Measurement of distance between receiver and constellation of satellites orbiting the Earth At any location there should be at least four of them ‘visible’ in the sky Powerful tool but conditions are not always ideal

    4. What’s in it for me? Opportunity to measure human navigational behaviour in a real-world setting Record position in space over time Objective and automatic measurements Practical potentials (e.g. blind mobility aid)

    5. Navigational processes Two main processes contribute to monitoring of position and orientation Landmark-based Monitoring changing position of distal visual cues Path integration Idiothetic cues (e.g. vestibular, kinaesthetic)

    6. Navigational processes Two main processes contribute to monitoring of position and orientation Landmark-based Monitoring changing position of distal visual cues Path integration Idiothetic cues (e.g. vestibular, kinaesthetic)

    7. Landmarks A fundamental source of information Serve to identify specific locations in our environment and enable calculation of position and heading First and most important aspect of our surroundings to be noticed and remembered as children develop large-scale environmental cognition (Piaget & Inhelder, 1967; Siegel & White, 1975)

    8. Reorientation Situations in which children seem unable to make use of landmarks Based on rat work by Cheng (1986)

    9. Human reorientation Hermer & Spelke (1994; 1996) reported similar findings with young children (around 2 years of age). Searched equally at the correct and geometrically equivalent corners even when the short wall adjacent to the target location was blue Children, like rats, reorient themselves using geometry ? impervious to landmark information and is therefore an innate and encapsulated module

    10. Human reorientation Hermer-Vasquez et al. (2001) used a regression approach to show that successful integration of landmarks and geometry for reorientation occurred with emergence of spatial relational grammar (5 – 7 years) Hermer-Vasquez et al. (1999) showed that adults failed to use landmarks to reorient when concurrently shadowing verbal material (successful with non-verbal rhythmic shadowing)

    11. Equivocal findings LANGUAGE Non-human animals including goldfish (Vargas et al., 2004), chicks (Vallortigara et al.,1990) and tamarins (Deipolyi et al., 2001) can integrate the two sources of information in the absence of language Verbal shadowing does not interfere with integration in adults when the condition order counterbalanced (Ratliff & Newcombe, 2005) ENVIRONMENT Children of successfully use landmarks in chamber twice the size of that used by Hermer & Spelke (Learmonth et al., 2001; 2002) Infants can also correctly combine geometry and landmarks in a rhombic environment (Hupbach & Nadel, 2005

    12. Environmental influence Hermer & Spelke space smaller than that in which children are used to navigating - likely crowded by people and goal locations Less navigational significance being attached to smaller or more portable objects (including the coloured fabric covering a short wall)

    13. Ecological validity Not established whether any of these theories scale up to the natural world - ultimate goal has been to relate the behaviour of humans to that of non-human animals in their natural environment (Wang & Spelke, 2002). Reorientation in the real world size of the environment can be assumed to approximate that in which homo-sapiens have evolved to negotiate landmarks more likely to be perceived as large, natural, and immutable role of geometry will likely be diminished, as the environment is less an enclosed or bounded space and more defined by the relationship between certain landmarks

    14. Experiment Focuses on utilisation of landmarks to reorient Three age groups (3/ 5/ 7 years) tested in a large open space (Durdham Downs) Smaller search space was created by a quadrilateral arrangement of four proximal hiding locations No geometric cues present to guide reorientation in either the arrangement of locations or in the open environment successful reorientation must be achieved by the use of landmarks.

    16. Experiment Four search trials in each arrangement, with every position used as the hiding location. Size of array counterbalanced - first four trials were with identical buckets, and the second four trials were with different colours Search recorded with GPS technology and Mobile Bristol software

    18. Mobile Bristol

    19. Results - accuracy t-tests against a chance performance level of 25% showed that all participants performed significantly above chance in each condition No effects of sex on any of the measures

    21. Results - latencies SMALL main effect of landmark colour (F(1, 29) = 5.12, p < .05) no between-subjects effect of age (F < 1) no age x colour interaction (F < 1) LARGE no effect of colour (F(1, 29) = 3.68, p > .05) no effect of age (F < 1) no age x colour interaction (F(2, 29) = 2.61, p > .05)

    22. Discussion Naturalistic landmarks are utilised by children as young as 3 years to reorient in a real world setting Utilisation is modulated by subtle environmental contexts - greater overall accuracy when the proximal locations were spaced in a large array (despite counterbalancing) Suggests that distal landmarks need to be successfully integrated with the proximal locations in order to be successfully utilised

    23. Discussion When search locations were further away they may have been processed as distal cues Maybe locations need to be coded in allocentric coordinates, at both proximal and distal spatial scales, in order to be integrated - might be more difficult for younger children, who are more likely to code locations in egocentric co-ordinates (Piaget & Inhelder, 1967; Siegel & White, 1975).

    24. Discussion Supports Learmonth et al. (2001) although based on the scale of the surrounding environment, rather than the search locations within it Counter to those reported by Hermer & Spelke Inadequacy in current accounts of human reorientation - Wang & Spelke (2002) model does not account for the nature of landmarks (proximal or distal) or encompass an allocentric representation of the environment that is separate from a distinct geometric boundary (Burgess, 2006).

    25. Navigational processes Two main processes contribute to monitoring of position and orientation Landmark-based Monitoring changing position of distal visual cues Path integration Idiothetic cues (e.g. vestibular, kinaesthetic)

    26. Ethological observations Widely studied in insects, birds and mammals Desert ants return to the nest using a novel and direct vector If transported in darkness they will perform appropriate homing vector to where nest should be Subject to forgetting – lost after sufficient delay

    27. Human path integration Humans also demonstrate the ability to integrate across vectors Walking to previously seen location (e.g. Philbeck et al., 1997) Path/ triangle completion (e.g. Loomis et al., 1993) Errors associated with path length and complexity (Klatzky et al., 1990)

    28. Dissociable components Homing vector gives a measure of distance and direction Suggestion that they are coded separately in locomotion (Berthoz et al. 1999) Neuropsychological evidence from RTL patients (Worsley et al., 2001)

    29. Individual differences Bredin et al. (2005) tested athletes and non-athletes on a distance estimation paradigm – athletes more accurate at fast velocities Athletic training improves spatial imagery – faster image transformation (Ozel et al., 2002) Athletes can more accurately navigate novel and complex environments at greater speeds (Gerin-Lajoie et al., 2007)

    30. Current experiment Bredin et al. (2005) state that practice in athletes develops and improves calibration of idiothetic information Does the spatial scale of that experience affect accuracy? Test athletes who perform at different spatial scales (rather than expert vs. novice) – rugby players (large-scale) and martial artists (small-scale) Tested in larger space than usually used in laboratory studies (remove some artefacts)

    31. Participants N=24 athletically trained males (19 – 43 years; M: 22.4) 12 martial artists from Judo and Jujitsu Societies at UoB 12 rugby players from UoB Men’s Rugby Club All trained for more than 7 hours a week All competed at University level

    32. Task

    33. Task

    34. Procedure Participants wear a GPS receiver on a belt around their waist Blindfolded and played white noise through headphones Passively led through leg 1, turned 90o, then led along leg 2. Tapped on shoulder when required to return to point of origin

    35. Mobile Bristol

    36. Results

    37. Distance error No effect of Group F (1, 21) = 1.70, p = .206 Significant Leg 1 x Leg 2 interaction F (4, 84) = 4.67, p < .005 No other main effects or interactions

    38. Angle error Main effect of Group F (2, 42) = 16.7, p = 0.001 Main effect of Leg 1 F (2, 42) = 6.36, p = 0.001 No other main effects or interactions

    39. Overall path length

    40. Discussion Difference between groups for angle/direction only – rugby players more accurate Greater experience of self-movement – better calibration of idiothetic cues? (i.e. Bredin et al., 2005) More experience of movement at a larger scale – continual reference to multiple elements of the game? Greater need to make inferences about spatial position without visual input?

    41. Discussion Increasing length of Leg 1 incurred more angular error – consistent with the outbound path being stored in memory (e.g. Fujita et al., 1993) Consistent with Worsley et al. (2001) – RTL patients impaired at angular estimation only Further evidence for dissociation of distance and direction information Effects appear to extend to a larger scale (although no analysis of under/overshoot)

    42. Conclusions A powerful means to record and track different components of navigational behaviour Affords a greater degree of accuracy than has hitherto been possible Next challenge is to develop the system further and to identify paradigms to be used Being independently developed by scientists in ostensibly different fields – a good forum for interbreeding

    43. Contact Dr. Alastair D. Smith School of Psychology University of Nottingham University Park Nottingham NG7 2RD UK Tel: +44 (0)115 951 5365 Fax: +44(0)115 951 5324 Email: alastair.smith@nottingham.ac.uk WWW: http://www.psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/staff/lpzas2/

More Related