1 / 25

Helping others

Helping others. Three true stories: One.

albert
Télécharger la présentation

Helping others

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Helping others

  2. Three true stories: One • In 1964, in the Queens district of New York City, Kitty Genovese was sexually assaulted and stabbed eight times. Overall the attack lasted 45 minutes, but in fact there were three separate attacks. The killer was scared off twice by Kitty's screams and struggles ("Oh my God, he stabbed me! Please help me! Please help me!"), and by the voices and lights of the 38 people who watched from their windows. As no-one intervened, however, he returned each time to attack again. No-one even called the police during the attack. One man phoned the police after Kitty was murdered, but he would not leave his name as he didn't want to get involved.

  3. Three true stories: Two • Joe Delaney, a Kansas City Chiefs football player, saw people standing around a huge water-filled hole. Three boys had waded in , unaware that a short way out the bottom dropped off. Suddenly they were in over their heads and thrashing and screaming for help. As Joe alone dashed for the pond, a little boy asked, "Can you swim?" "I can't swim good," Joe answered, "but I've got to save those kids. If I don't come up, get somebody." One boy struggled back to safety. Later the other two - and Joe Delaney - were hauled out by rescuers, dead.

  4. Three true stories: Three • During the second world war the SS Dorchester was torpedoed by the Nazi submarine U-456. There were four chaplains on board: Methodist George Fox; Rabbi Alexander Goode; Catholic John Washington; and Reformed Church minister Clark Poling. As the four chaplains arrived on the dark and steeply sloping deck they began to guide other men to their boat stations. They distributed life jackets and coaxed men over the side. In the icy, oil-smeared water, Pvt. William Bednar heard the chaplains preaching courage and found the strength to swim out until he reached a life-raft. Still on board, Grady Clark watched in awe as the chaplains handed out the last life jackets and then gave away their own. As Clark slipped into the waters he saw the chaplains standing - their arms linked-praying, in Latin, Hebrew, and English. Other men, now serene, joined them in a huddle as the Dorchester slid beneath the sea.

  5. Lecture contents • A reminder of evolutionary psychology. • Helping from an evolutionary perspective. • The altruism question. • A sequential decision-making framework for helping. • The effect of standards (models and norms). • Individual (personality) differences.

  6. Evolutionary psychology: A reminder • “If a specific social behaviour enhances reproductive success, the genetic underpinnings of that behaviour are more likely to be passed on to subsequent generations” • (Brehm et al., 2002, p. 347) • “If...” • (i) This does not mean that alternative behaviours may not also be successful. • (ii) Specific behaviours are fit for specific environments. • “...the genetic underpinnings of that behaviour...” • (i) If any.

  7. Evolution and helping • Helping is typically more costly than non-helping. • Non-helping more ‘fit’ and so should become increasingly prevalent in subsequent generations. • How then can evolutionary theory explain helping? • (i) Claim that helping has no genetic basis. • (ii) Deny that helping is necessarily more costly than non-helping. • Kinship selection. • Reciprocal altruism. • Group selection.

  8. Burnstein et al. (1994) Preferential helping of relatives in trouble

  9. As you sow, so shall your genes reap • Kinship selection • Just discussed. • Reciprocal altruism • Helping someone increases our probability of being helped back when needed. • Group selection • Helping the group may be repaid by virtue of receiving the benefits of being a group member.

  10. Psychological (motivational) altruism Baston (1991) The empathy-altruism hypothesis

  11. The empathy-altruism paradigm and predictions

  12. Batson et al. (1981) Empathy-induced altruism

  13. Reward-seeking egoistic alternatives • Negative state relief model(Cialdini et al., 1987) • High empathy increases sadness that can be relieved by obtaining the rewards of helping • Will only help others when doing so is necessary and effective for reducing negative personal affect, e.g., sadness. • Empathic joy(Smith et al. 1989) • High empathy makes possible obtaining personal satisfaction when the needy other’s welfare is improved. • Will only help others when doing so is necessary and effective for increasing positive personal affect, e.g., joy.

  14. Latané & Darley (1970) Five steps to emergency-intervention

  15. Step 1: Noticing • Piliavin et al. (1976) • More help when dramatic event witnessed rather than just aftermath of it. • Mathews & Canon (1975) • More help without stimulus-overload (cf. Milgram, 1970). • McMillen et al. (1977) • More help when in an ‘externally-attentive’ good mood than in a ‘self-absorbed’ bad one.

  16. Step 2: Interpreting • Latané & Darley (1968) • Pluralistic ignorance can lead to reduced probability of an individual helping when in the presence of others and exposed to an ambiguous need for help. • Clark & Word (1972) • When the need is not ambiguous, the presence of others has no effect on helping levels. • Staub (1974) • The cues others give can reduce or increase the probability of an individual bystander giving help.

  17. Step 3: Taking personal responsibility • Latané & Darley (1968) • The chances of any given participant helping in the “seizure study” decreased as numbers of observers seemingly increased. • Diffusion of responsibility - Taking less personal responsibility because one believes that others will (or should) provide help. • Bickman (1971) • Responsibility not diffused when co-witnesses are clearly not able to help. • Moriarty (1975) • Responsibility not diffused when specifically attached to a bystander.

  18. Steps 4 & 5: Choosing how to help, and Acting • Shotland & Heinold (1985) • College students trained in first-aid were not more likely to help someone with profuse arterial bleeding. • They were, however, much more likely to do the right thing! • Remember: sometimes the most helpful thing one can do is not provide direct assistance (cf. overhelping, Gilbert & Silvera, 1996). • Which of course adds to the ambiguity of whether and what help is need.

  19. Modelling helpfulness • Ruston (1975) • Actions speak louder than words - for a while

  20. The altruistic personality • There are individual differences in helpfulness across situations and times. • E.g. ‘Rescuers’ and 60s civil right activists were more helpful than than non-rescuers and non-activists 20-40 years later. • Rushton et al. (1984) • Genetic basis to this individual difference • In terms of “behavioural tendenices” and “helping-related emotions and reactions (such as empathy”), monozygotic twins are more similar to each other than are dizygotic twins.

  21. Rescuers, relative to non-rescuers (Oliner & Oliner, 1988) • Greater actual and perceived similarity to Jews. • More likely to have been directly asked to help. • Parents more caring, much less likely to use punishment, and more likely to reason and explain. • Much more likely to strongly identify with a highly moral (and morally acting) parent. • More dispositional empathy. • More willing to accept responsibility for own actions and others’ welfare. • More extensivity (attachment to and empathy, concern, and responsibility for outgroup members). • Higher self-efficacy.

  22. Ordinary helpers, relative to non-helpers, have • More dispositional empathy • Davis (1983) • Correlation between empathy and donations to a telethon. • Greater sense of responsibility • Berkowitz & Daniels (1964) • People high in social responsibility more likely to help needy dependents. • ‘Other-oriented empathy’/extensivity • Penner et al. (1995) • Correlation between this and length of service as a volunteer. • Greater self-efficacy • Rushton (1984) • Volunteers particularly high in self-efficacy.

  23. Interaction • Staub (1974) • Those high in social responsibility and other-oriented moral reasoning were more likely to come to the aid of an ill person. • Penner et al. (1995) • Those high in ‘other-oriented empathy’ and self-efficacy more helpful than those high in one or neither characteristic. • Miller et al. (1996) • (“Cold”) moral reasoning and (“hot”) empathic arousal may be necessary for self-sacrificing prosocial action (see next slide).

  24. Miller et al. (1996) Interactive effects of children’s empathy and moral reasoning

  25. Interactionism • Snyder (1992) • Strong situational cues often reduce the impact of personality. Personality flourishes when situation cues weak. • Carlo et al. (1991) • Used a procedure close to Batson’s ‘Elaine’ one. • Most people (78%) helped in difficult escape situation, with little difference according to extensivity-type measure. • In easy escape condition, 49% helped, with high-extensives more likely to help than low-extensives. • Wilson (1976) • In emergency situation, personality variables related to arousal, emotionality, and risk-taking predict helping. • Batson et al. (1976) • In non-emergency situations, personality variables related to cost-benefit concerns, confidence, and self-esteem predict helping.

More Related