1 / 62

Circular analysis in systems neuroscience – with particular attention to cross-subject correlation mapping

Circular analysis in systems neuroscience – with particular attention to cross-subject correlation mapping. Nikolaus Kriegeskorte Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health. Collaborators. Chris I Baker W Kyle Simmons Patrick SF Bellgowan Peter Bandettini.

nairi
Télécharger la présentation

Circular analysis in systems neuroscience – with particular attention to cross-subject correlation mapping

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience– with particular attention to cross-subject correlation mapping Nikolaus Kriegeskorte Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health

  2. Collaborators Chris I Baker W Kyle Simmons Patrick SF Bellgowan Peter Bandettini

  3. Overview Part 1General introduction to circular analysis in systems neuroscience(synopsis of Kriegeskorte et al. 2009) Part 2Specific issue: selection bias incross-subject correlation mapping(following up on Vul et al. 2009)

  4. data results

  5. analysis data results

  6. analysis data results

  7. assumptions analysis data results

  8. assumptions data results analysis

  9. Circular inference assumptions analysis data results

  10. Circular inference assumptions analysis data results

  11. Weighting (continuous selection) Elimination (binary selection) Sorting (multiclass selection) How do assumptions tinge results? – Through variants of selection!

  12. Elimination (binary selection) assumptions: selection criteria analysis data results

  13. Example 1Pattern-information analysis

  14. Experimental design TASK (property judgment) Simmons et al. 2006 “Animate?” “Pleasant?” STIMULUS (object category)

  15. Pattern-information analysis define ROI by selecting ventral-temporal voxels for which any pairwise condition contrast is significant at p<.001 (uncorr.) perform nearest-neighbor classificationbased on activity-pattern correlation use oddruns for trainingand evenruns for testing

  16. Results stimulus (object category) task (judged property) decoding accuracy chance level 1 0.5 0

  17. stimulus task decoding accuracy chance level ! ? fMRI data data from Gaussian random generator using all data to select ROI voxels 1 1 1 1 ...but we used cleanly independent training and test data! using only training data to select ROI voxels 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0 0 0 0

  18. Conclusion for pattern-information analysis The test data must not be used in either... • training a classifier or • defining the ROI continuous weighting binary weighting

  19. Data selection is key to many conventional analyses. Can it entail similar biases in other contexts?

  20. Example 2Regional activation analysis

  21. ROI definition is affected by noise independent ROI overfitted ROI true region overestimated effect ROI-average activation

  22. Data sorting assumptions: sorting criteria analysis data results

  23. Set-average tuning curves ...for data sorted by tuning response stimulus parameter (e.g. orientation) noise data

  24. ROI-average fMRI response A B C D condition Set-average activation profiles ...for data sorted by activation noise data

  25. To avoid selection bias, we can... ...perform a nonselective analysis OR ...make sure that selection and results statistics are independent under the null hypothesis, because they are either: • inherently independent • or computed on independent data e.g. whole-brain mapping (no ROI analysis) e.g. independent contrasts

  26. Does selection by an orthogonal contrast vector ensure unbiased analysis? cselection=[1 1]T ctest=[1 -1]T orthogonal contrast vectors  ROI-definition contrast: A+B ROI-average analysis contrast: A-B

  27. Does selection by an orthogonal contrast vector ensure unbiased analysis? contrast vector – No, there can still be bias. still not sufficient not sufficient The design and noise dependencies matter. design noise dependencies

  28. Circular analysis Pros Cons • highly sensitive • widely accepted (examples in all high-impact journals) • doesn't require independent data sets • grants scientists independencefrom the data • allows smooth blending of blind faith and empiricism

  29. Circular analysis Pros Cons • highly sensitive • widely accepted (examples in all high-impact journals) • doesn't require independent data sets • grants scientists independencefrom the data • allows smooth blending of blind faith and empiricism

  30. Circular analysis Pros Pros Cons [can’t think of any right now] • the error that beautifies results • confirms even incorrect hypotheses • improves chances ofhigh-impact publication • highly sensitive • widely accepted (examples in all high-impact journals) • doesn't require independent data sets • grants scientists independencefrom the data • allows smooth blending of blind faith and empiricism

  31. Part 2Specific issue: selection bias incross-subject correlation mapping(following up on Vul et al. 2009)

  32. Motivation Vul et al. (2009) posed a puzzle: Why are the cross-subject correlations found in brain mapping so high? Selection bias is one piece of the puzzle. But there are more pieces and we have yet to put them all together.

  33. Overview • List and discuss six pieces of the puzzle. (They don't all point in the same direction!) • Suggest some guidelines for good practice.

  34. Six pieces synopsis • Cross-subject correlation estimates are very noisy. • Bin or within-subject averaging legitimately increases correlations. • Selecting among noisy estimates yields large biases. • False-positive regions are highly likely for a whole-brain mapping thresholded at p<.001, uncorrected. • Reported correlations are high, but not highly significant. • Studies have low power for finding realistic correlations in the brain if multiple testing is appropriately accounted for.

  35. Vul et al. 2009 ,, noise-free correlation population ,, The geometric mean of the reliability is an upper bound on the population correlation. The reliabilities provide no bound on the sample correlation.

  36. Piece 1 Sample correlationsacross small numbers of subjectsare very noisy estimatesof population correlations.

  37. 0.65

  38. Cross-subject correlation estimatesare very noisy 95%-confidence interval correlation 10 subjects

  39. Cross-subject correlation estimatesare very noisy

  40. Piece 2 The more we average(reducing noise but not signal),the higher correlations become.

  41. Bin-averaging inflates correlations

  42. Bin-averaging inflates correlations

  43. Subjects are like bins... For each subject, all data is averaged to give one number. Take-home message Cross-subject correlation estimates are expected to be... • high (averaging all data for each subject) • noisy (low number of subjects) So what's Ed fussing about?We don't need selection bias to explain the high correlations, right?

  44. Piece 3 Selecting the maximumamong noisy estimatesyields large selection biases.

  45. Expected maximum correlationselected among null regions expected maximum correlation bias 16 subjects

  46. Piece 4 False-positive regions are likely to be found in whole-brain mappingusing p<.001, uncorrected.

  47. Mapping with p<.001, uncorrected Global null hypothesis is true (population correlation = 0 in all brain locations)

More Related