1 / 21

The conditioned stimulus

The conditioned stimulus. Generalization: A range of stimuli varying on a particular dimension will produce responses in proportion to how similar they are to the trained CS. The generalization gradient is the graph of the strength of the CR to stimuli varying above and below the CS.

abrianna
Télécharger la présentation

The conditioned stimulus

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. The conditioned stimulus • Generalization: A range of stimuli varying on a particular dimension will produce responses in proportion to how similar they are to the trained CS. • The generalization gradient is the graph of the strength of the CR to stimuli varying above and below the CS.

  2. A generalization gradient

  3. Discrimination learning • If stimuli other than the CS are presented, but never paired with the US, the generalized responses to similar stimuli will diminish, so that the individual discriminates between CS and non-CS. • Examples of classically-conditioned discrimination learning abound in education: colors, shapes, letters...The color or shape or letter is CS, the teacher’s word for it is the US.

  4. Results of discrimination learning Generalization around a light CS 40 30 20 Eyeblinks per minute 10 0 470 480 490 500 510 520 530 Wavelength of light, nm (Color)

  5. The nature of the CR • The CR is always different from the UR. • Sometimes the CR differs from the UR only in magnitude, duration, or latency. • But sometimes, the CR is opposite to the UR: Relaxation and slowed heart rate as a CR to a CS for shock, but agitation and increased heart rate as a UR to the actual shock US.

  6. Opponent Process Theory • Opponent processes are bodily reactions opposite to the effect of a US, in order to preserve physiological balance. • If a CS is conditioned to a US, the opponent process rather than the UR becomes the CR. • Wagner (1981) argued that opponent processes are conditioned as CRs only for URs which provoke compensatory reactions, but not for monophasic URs: SOP.

  7. Applications of SOP • Drug tolerance • CS (setting for drug taking) + US (drug) ----> CR (tolerance) • US (drug) without CS (setting) ----> Overdose • Spousal boredom • CS (setting: Usual routine) + US (spouse) ----> CR (boredom) • US (spouse) without CS (new setting) ----> Excitement

  8. Contiguity vs. contingency • The S-S and S-R theories we have studied operate on the associationist principle of contiguity: Research on CS-US intervals. • A competing view is called contingency: differential prediction of the US by the CS, not mere co-occurrence. • In a contingent relationship, p(US | CS) > p(US | not-CS)

  9. Rescorla and contingency learning • The greater the differential contingency, the greater the learning: (Correct legend p.59) • The suppression ratio: (A - P)/(A + P) • A is the response rate in the Absence of the CS • P is the response rate in the Presence of the CS • If A = P, there is no suppression, and the ratio = 0 • If A > P, there is suppression, expressed in the ratio. • Thus, the higher the ratio, the greater the suppression, and the greater the learning.

  10. Contiguity or contingency: Rescorla (1968) • Notice that the greater the difference between the pUS|CS and the pUS|CS, the greater the suppression ratio: Contingency.

  11. Occasion setting and conditioned inhibition • Conditioned inhibition: Summation paradigm • Separately condition CS+ to US and CS- to not-US • Test CS+ and CS- together • Amount of reduction of CR shows strength of CS- • Conditioned inhibition: Retardation paradigm • First condition CS- to not-US • Then, keeping CS-, try to condition CS+ to US • Amount of retardation of conditioning shows strength of CS- • Occasion setting • Clocks and kisses

  12. Preparedness and associative bias: Garcia • Shock US conditions better to light/tone CS than to taste CS • Nausea-inducing US drug conditions better to taste CS than to light/tone CS • Possible conclusion: Associations are reasonable inferences

  13. Stimulus combinations • Kamin (1968): Informativeness, blocking, and unblocking Group 1: CS1 + CS2 ---> US (eight trials) Group 2: CS1 ---> US (sixteen trials) CS1 + CS2 ---> US (eight trials) Group difference? • Configural cue learning: not individual cue learning: A + B but not A or B; A or B but not A + B; [A+C and B+D] but not [A + C or B + D]

  14. Rescorla-Wagner theory • The theory has an equation with three parts: • V, the amount of learning acquired • a , the rate of learning • l , the maximum amount of learning that can happen to the particular US. • Together, DV = a ( l - V) • The equation is applied once for each learning trial, to see how much learning will happen on each trial.

  15. Example of Rescorla-Wagner computations • DV = a ( l - V) • If a = .20 and l = 100, work through 4 trials: • Trial 1: .20 ( 100 - 0) = 20 • Trial 2: .20 ( 100 - 20) = 16 • Trial 3: .20 ( 100 - 36) = 12.8 • Trial 4: .20 ( 100 - 48.8) = 10.24 • Note the diminishing returns with repeated trials. Will learning ever stop?

  16. Another example • DV = a ( l - V) • If a = .10 and l = 200, work through 5 trials: • Trial 1: .10 ( 200 - 0) = 20 • Trial 2: .10 ( 200 - 20) = 18 • Trial 3: .10 ( 200 - 38) = 16.2 • Trial 4: .10 ( 200 - 54.2) = 14.58 • Trial 5: .10 ( 200 - 68.78) = 13.12 • In a relative sense, learning is happening more slowly here.

  17. A learning curve from Rescorla-Wagner theory

  18. Rescorla-Wagner and compound stimuli • Competitive learning: The total learning available, l , must be shared by each stimulus in a compound. Thus, the total amount of learning to each stimulus is less in a compound than if that stimulus is alone. • Rescorla-Wagner predicts blocking and conditioned inhibition accurately.

  19. Problems with Rescorla-Wagner • CS preexposure produces slower conditioning to CS later, a phenomenon known as latent inhibition. For example, displaying the letters of the alphabet around the classroom before they are learned is CS preexposure. • Latent inhibition is not predicted by Rescorla-Wagner, unless you assume that preexposure lowers the learning rate (a) by lowering salience.

  20. Connectionism: A Neural model • Learning may involve forming patterns of synaptic connections. • ABCD --> CR1, ie US1 • ABCF --> CR2, ie US2 • The rule for connection formation is the delta rule, based on Rescorla-Wagner • CSs are inputs; USs are outputs.

  21. Applications of classical conditioning • Classroom structure • Make salient what is to be learned • Eliminate redundant CSs • Establish differential contingency • Extinction • Counterconditioning • Desensitization • Flooding

More Related