1 / 81

GRS LX 700 Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory

GRS LX 700 Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory. Week 13. Models, input, intake, attrition. The Monitor Hypothesis.

derora
Télécharger la présentation

GRS LX 700 Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. GRS LX 700Language Acquisition andLinguistic Theory Week 13.Models, input, intake, attrition

  2. The Monitor Hypothesis • A linguistic expression originates in the system of acquired knowledge, but prior to output a “Monitor” checks it against consciously known rules and may modify the expression before it is uttered. Learned competence (the Monitor) Acquired competence output

  3. What makes input into intake? • Apperception: Recognizing the gap between what L2’er knows and what there is to know. • Comprehensibility: Either the semantic meaning is determinable or the relevant structural aspects are determinable. • Attention: Selecting aspects of the knowledge to be learned (from among many other possible things) for processing. • Output: Forcing a structural hypothesis, elsewhere used to shape input into a form useful for intake.

  4. Input  apperception • Some input is apperceived, some isn’t. • That which isn’t is thought of as blocked by various “filters”: • Time pressure • Frequency non-extremes • Affective (status, motivation, attitude, …) • Prior knowledge (grounding, analyzability) • Salience (drawing attention)

  5. Apperception  comprehension • Modification of speech to learner (“foreigner talk”) • Redundancy • Negotiation for meaning • (often, meaning is a precursor to being able to assign a syntactic representation). • Note: Much of the following discussion is probably more about learning than acquisition. Cf. “L2A” of C++.

  6. Foreigner talk • Like the better-known phenomenon of “baby talk”, it also turns out that people conversing with others whom they perceive to be non-native speakers (NNSs) will often use a form of “foreigner talk”—modified language forms presumably intended to simplify the utterance.

  7. Foreigner talk • Slower, clearer articulation • Higher frequency vocabulary, fewer idioms • Providing more definitions • Less elliptical • More gestures • Short, simple sentences • Moving topics to the front of the sentence, new information to the end of the sentence • More repetition, restatements. • Recasting NNS’s incorrect statements

  8. Foreigner talk • The ways in which this happens varies a lot—where it happens at all, there are many different ways that sentences are “simplified”. • The adjustments often happen in the face of an evident lack of comprehension.

  9. Foreigner talk • NNS: How have increasing food costs changed your eating habits? • NS: Well, I don’t know that it’s changed them. I try to adjust. • NNS: Pardon me? • NS: I don’t think it’s changed my eating habits.

  10. Foreigner talk • NNS: How have increasing food costs changed your eating habits? • NS: Oh, rising costs… we’ve cut back on the more expensive things. Gone to cheaper foods. • NNS: Pardon me? • NS: We’ve gone to cheaper foods.

  11. Foreigner talk • The “simplification” sometimes even sacrifices grammaticality, which is probably of dubious value both for comprehension and learning. • Basil: It’s not fire; it’s only bell. • NNS has an object from a grab-bag, NS is trying to guess its identity. • NS: Ok, little guy! Yeah, yours! Okay! Yours is it for eat? • NNS: Eat. No.

  12. Foreigner talk • A: Yesterday my country change, ah, President. • NS: Oh yeah? Now, is the new one a good one? • A: Um? • NS: Is a good President? Do you like him? No? • NS: Does she speak English? • C: No. • NS: Nothing? • C: No. • NS: She doesn’t talk? Always quiet? No talk?

  13. Comprehension • In general, this appears to be in service of comprehension—done in order to make linguistically less sophisticated interlocutors able to understand. • Once there is understanding, we also are ready for there to be intake of the input as well.

  14. “Backchannel cues” • L2’ers often foil this process by providing “backchannel cues” which indicate to the NS that communication is proceeding, comprehension has been achieved. “Smile and nod”.

  15. I’d like to buy a TV. • NNS is trying to buy a TV, but accidentally called a repair shop. • … • Ah Sony please. • We don’t work on Sonys. • Or Sylvania. • Sylvania? • Uh huh. • Oh, Sylvania, OK. That’s American made. • OK.

  16. I’d like to buy a TV. • All right. Portables have to be brought in. • Hm hm. • And there’s no way I can tell you how much it’ll cost until he looks at it. • Hm hm. • And it’s a $12.50 deposit. • OK. • And if he can fix it that applies to labor and if he can’t he keeps the $12.50 for his time and effort. • Hm hm.

  17. I’d like to buy a TV. • How old of a TV is it? Do you know off hand? • 19 inch. • How old of a TV is it? Is it a very old one or only a couple years old? • Oh, so so. • The only thing you can do is bring it in and let him look at it and go from there. • New television please.

  18. Comprehension vs. output • Comprehension can come in various ways, some of which have nothing to do with the structure. • With some knowledge of the situation, and assuming speaker will make sense, be relevant, provide given and new information appropriately, be cooperative, the listener can come quite close to understanding the meaning without having any kind of syntactic analysis for it. • If learning the structure of the target language is considered to be the ultimate goal, this kind of comprehension may be unhelpful.

  19. Comprehension vs. output • No such crutches are available for production, however. If you’re going to say something in the target language, you’ll need to choose a syntax. • Output viewed this way could be a way of creating grammatical knowledge (not just using pre-existing knowledge)—forcing an analysis where there was not one before.

  20. Output and negative evidence • Of course, output will give the L2’er practice, allow for the “automation” of certain things allowing attention to shift elsewhere. • Additionally, output provides an opportunity for negative evidence, correction from the outside.

  21. Negotiating for meaning • Very often a NS-NNS (or NNS-NNS) conversation will involve a fair amount of negotiating for meaning—where understanding has not happened, the conversation takes a detour to repair the problem.

  22. Negotiating for meaning • (S) Had to declare—declare?—her ingress. • (J) English? • No. English no (laugh)… ingress, her ingress. • Ingless? • Ingress. Yes. I N G R E S S more or less. • Ingless. • Yes. If for example, if you, when you work you had an ingress, you know? • Uh huh an ingless? • Yes..

  23. Negotiating for meaning • Uh huh OK • Yes, if for example, your homna, husband works, when finish, when end the month his job, his boss pay—mm—him something. • Aaaah. • And your family have some ingress. • Yes, ah, ok ok. • More or less ok? And in this institution take care of all ingress of the company and review the accounts. • Ok I got, I see. • Ok. My father work there, but now he is old.

  24. Pre-empting negotiation • In the category of “foreigner talk” we might also include these… • Lots of comprehension/confirmation checks and clarification requests: • I was born in Nagasaki. Do you know Nagasaki? • And your family have some ingress…more or less ok? • (When can you go to visit me?) Visit? • (…research). Research, I don’t know the meaning. • Questions often come with suggested responses • When do you take the break? At ten-thirty?

  25. Healthy miscommunication • A failure to communicate can serve to focus attention on areas where the NNS’s grammar is non-native-like, prompting negotiation for meaning and providing possible intake data. • Michael Long’s “Interaction Hypothesis” is that this kind of negotiation for meaning and resulting attention is necessary for advancement toward the grammar of the target grammar—in part because it connects input, existing knowledge, selective attention, and output in productively trying to solve a current language deficiency.

  26. Still, feedback isn’t everything • Ideally, a learner produces an ungrammatical sentences, gets negative feedback indicating that there is a problem (those involved in the conversation negotiate for meaning), focusing attention on the problem area, and the learner takes input bearing on this as intake, incorporating it into his/her grammar.

  27. Still, feedback isn’t everything • Problem is, such evidence is not very consistent—it might be helpful when it happens, but it’s hard to be sure when it is happening. • First: Not all incorrect forms get corrected (e.g., if the hearer understood). • Second: Errors leading to misunderstanding might not be revealed until quite a bit later, if at all…

  28. Still, feedback isn’t everything • NS: When I get to Paris, I’m going to sleep for one whole day. I’m so tired. • NNS: What? • NS: I’m going to sleep for one whole day. • NNS: One hour a day? • NS: Yes. • NNS: Why? • NS: Because I’m so tired. • …enduring silence…

  29. Still, feedback isn’t everything • Moreover, how is this useful feedback? • NS: Did you fly to Singapore yesterday? • NNS: Did I flied here yesterday? • NS: Pardon? • NNS: Did I flied here yesterday? • How does this help fix the problem?

  30. Still, feedback isn’t everything • Sometimes it works… • NNS: There is a library. • NS: A what? • NNS: A place where you put books. • NS: A bookshelf? • NNS: He pass his house • NS: Sorry? • NNS: He passed, he passed, ah, his sign.

  31. Still, feedback isn’t everything • Feedback (“negative evidence”) is just too inconsistent to be reliable—to really be the whole story about how people learn a second language. • Interaction does seem to help, though, for whatever reason…

  32. Mackey 1999 • Looked at question formation in ESL speakers. • Tasks (designed to spark questions) • Story completion • Working out a story by asking questions • Picture sequencing • Discovering the order of a picture story • Picture differences • Identifying the differences between similar pictures • Picture drawing • Describing or drawing a picture.

  33. Mackey 1999: Procedure

  34. Measure of development using question formation • 2: SVO? • Your cat is black? • 3: Fronting Wh/Do • What the cat doing in your picture? • 4: Inversion (auxiliaries not do) • Have you drawn the cat? • 5: Do/Aux-second (wh-front, inversion) • Why have you left home? • 6: Uninverted in embedded clauses • Can you tell me where the cat is?

  35. Mackey 1999: subject groups • Interactors • Natural interaction • Interactor unreadies • Natural interaction, but low development measure • Observers • Watch interactionally modified input, answer comprehension questions afterwards. • Scripteds • Premodified (scripted) input, leaving very little room for communication breakdown. • Controls

  36. Mackey 1999 • Scripted (premodified) group • NS: and now under it draw a pear. A pear is a fruit. It is like an apple. The color is green. Draw the pear under the book. Can you draw it? • NNS: Ok ok I got it. Look like apple (draws) • NS: Good. Now on the right of the pear draw an umbrella.

  37. Mackey 1999 • Interactor groups • NS: Underneath it is a pear, it’s green • NNS: What is it a bear? • NS: A pear, pears are fruit, it’s a fruit, juicy like an apple • NNS: Ok pear, fruit like Japanese fruit nashi very delicious. You saw this in Japan? Have you eat one? • NS: Yeah I did but a nashi is round yeah? Pears are round on the bottom, narrow on top. Have you eaten one here in Australia? • NNS: Yes thank you. I had a pear in my lunch (time) not…juicy? (draws) Like this?

  38. Mackey 1999 • Looking at whether different groups moved up a developmental stage. • Must produce at least 2 higher-level questions in 2 of the 3 post-tests to have “moved up a stage” • Private ESL school in Australia, about 6 months in residence overall. • 27 classified as lower-intermediate, 7 classified as beginners (the interactor unreadies), average 1.7 mo. in residence.

  39. Mackey 1999: % people moved up a stage

  40. Interactors significantly more likely to move up

  41. Mackey 1999: Increase in stage 4 & 5 questions in posttests

  42. Mackey 1999 • Mackey claims that her study shows that the interactors have a significant advantage over the non-interactors, based on the previous graph (production of high-stage questions). • Yet—should advancing stage be the real goal? In that, the observers also benefited.

  43. Mackey 1999 • Delayed benefit— • Interestingly in the latest posttest (2 weeks after the treatment), the numbers of high-stage questions had continued to grow. • Suggests perhaps that this had focused attention on areas that needed work, but the grammatical changes were not implemented immediately.

  44. Input, interaction… UG? • UG hasn’t played a very big role in the discussion of the importance of interaction, converting input into intake, negotiating for meaning. How can we connect them?

  45. Parameters, triggers • Recall that one of the crucial features of parameters is that (ideally) each parameter setting has a cluster of effects. • It’s not just that the verb appears before adverbs—it is that the verb moves into the tense position, which means it appears before adverbs and before negation. Coming before adverbs and coming before negation are a cluster of properties tied to the single verb-raising parameter.

  46. Parameters, triggers • In order to set a parameter in the way which matches the setting reflected by the language in the environment, the learner needs to look for consequences of a particular setting. • Designated bits of data which can serve as unambiguous indicators of one parameter setting over another are sometimes called triggers.

  47. Parameters, triggers • So, for example, the L1’er’s task is to examine the input for instances of these triggers and use them to set the parameter to the correct value. • Some of the consequences of any given parameter setting might be fairly obscure, not likely to show up in frequent (or easily analyzed) ambient speech data accessible to the kid. This might make it hard to set one’s parameters—but for the clustering property.

  48. Parameters, triggers • Indications that the verb moves: • Seeing verbs before negation • Seeing verbs before adverbs • Indications that the verb doesn’t move: • Do-support (The verb does not usually move) • Indications that null subjects are allowed: • Null subjects are observed. • Postverbal subjects are allowed. • Indications that null subjects are not allowed: • Expletive subjects are observed (it’s raining).

  49. Parameters, triggers • If triggers are what setting parameters is all about, then the interaction stuff is probably about making the triggers more salient. • Unfortunately, it is difficult to interpret existing “input enhancement” type studies in these terms because they measured different things—we don’t know what triggers were present, what effect making triggers (vs. non-triggers?) salient had.

  50. Parameters, triggers • If language acquisition (first or second) were just about finding triggers to set the parameters, why is it so hard then? Why is negotiation, etc. important (to L2A anyway)? This suggests that the triggers are in the “incomprehensible” input, that needs to be elaborated on in order to be used as intake (and thus to set the parameter).

More Related