1 / 49

Learning Alternation without Bias

Learning Alternation without Bias . Young Ah Do MIT youngah@mit.edu WCCFL 29 April. 22-24. 2011 . Learning the Pattern of Alternation . Alternation: the realization of the same morpheme in various phonological contexts. l ike[ t ] buzze [ d ] hunt[ ɪd ]

madra
Télécharger la présentation

Learning Alternation without Bias

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Learning Alternation without Bias Young Ah Do MIT youngah@mit.edu WCCFL 29 April. 22-24. 2011

  2. Learning the Pattern of Alternation • Alternation: the realization of the same morpheme in various phonological contexts. • like[t] buzze[d] hunt[ɪd] • Learning alternation • Korean noun inflection Young Ah Do, MIT

  3. Korean Noun Paradigm • Stem-final obstruents • UR /k’oth/ (Ko 1989) • [k’ot] ‘flower’ • [k’oth-ɨl]~[k’os-ɨl] [k’och-ɨl], [k’oc-ɨl] ‘flower-acc’ (Hayes 1998, Albright 2005, Choe 2004) • Stem-final nonobstruents • UR /pal/ • [pal] ‘foot’ • [paɾ-ɨl] ‘foot-acc’ intersonorant flapping Young Ah Do, MIT

  4. Learning Challenge: Obstruent–final Nouns • Phonotactically unpredictable • An order of usage frequency • s >>ch, th>> c, t (Jun 2007) • Different preference for each suffix • ch-ɨl >> th-il vs. th-e>> ch-e (Jun 2007) • Type frequency highest for lateral final nouns. (Kenstowicz & Sohn 2007) Young Ah Do, MIT

  5. Learning Stages of Alternations • Children (4;2-7;8) produce adult inflection of obstruent-final nouns slower than nonobstruent-final nouns (Do, ms). • Nonobsruent-final nouns: Identical inflectional form across all age groups. • Obstruent-final nouns: Three-stage learning Young Ah Do, MIT

  6. Production of Obstruent-final Nouns • Three-stage learning • Adults : Variations [k’os-ɨl] ~ [k’och-ɨl] ~ [k’oth-ɨl]‘flower-acc’ • 4;2-5;6 : Attempt an isolation form [k’ot] • 6;2-7;8 : Most frequent adult form [k’os-ɨl] Young Ah Do, MIT

  7. Goal • Predict three-stage learning by training a learner with • Constraint-based grammar • The statistical distribution of alternations without • Assuming intrinsic bias Young Ah Do, MIT

  8. Overview • Alternations in Korean noun paradigms and three-stage learning • Analysis • The initial stage is due to incorrectly promoted Output-Output Faithfulness constraints (McCarthy 1999). • The intermediate stage results from demoting Output-Output Faithfulness constraints (OO-F). Young Ah Do, MIT

  9. Overview • MaxEnt Grammar Tool (Hayes 2009) • Simulation • Constraints: OO-F, Markedness, and IO-F constraints. • Type frequency of adult variants in corpus • Learning alternations without bias Young Ah Do, MIT

  10. Korean Noun Paradigm • Three-way laryngeal contrast among obstruents (Jun 2009, p.3) Young Ah Do, MIT

  11. Alternations in Nouns • Neutralization of obstruents to their homorganic lenis stop counterparts in coda position • /suph/ [sup] ‘forest’ • /path/ [pat] ‘field’ • /puəkh/ [puək] ‘kitchen’ Young Ah Do, MIT

  12. Variation in Nouns • Prevocalic allomorphs of the noun stems show variation in final obstruents. Young Ah Do, MIT

  13. Observations about Variation • Variants: [s, th, ch, c, t], excluding tense consonants. • [s] >> [ch], [th] >> [c] , [t] • [ch]-ɨX >> [th]-ɨX • [th]-eX >> [ch]-eX • Relative frequency of variants matches corpus frequency (Jun 2009). Young Ah Do, MIT

  14. Experimental Results • Adults • Variation matchescorpus frequency • Young (4;2-5;6) • Attempt unmarked forms. • [pas-ɨl] ‘field-acc’  [pat], [pat an-ɨl] ‘field inside-acc’ Young Ah Do, MIT

  15. Alternation at Initial Stage Young Ah Do, MIT

  16. Alternation at Intermediate Stage • Intermediate (6;2-7;8) • Attempt the most frequent variant among adult forms. • Nom: [s-i] • Acc: [s-ɨl] • Loc: [s-e], [th-e] • Source: [th-esə] Young Ah Do, MIT

  17. Alternation at Intermediate Stage Young Ah Do, MIT

  18. Learning Path • The pattern of production Young: Unmarked form  Intermediate: The most frequent variant  Adults: Variants with different preference Young Ah Do, MIT

  19. The 1st stage: Unmarked form • The unmarked form is the most frequent in child directed speech • Unmarked 75%, Nom 20%, Acc 5% (I. Lee 1999) • But, this is not a frequency effect. • Children do know both unmarked and correct inflected forms. • Children innovate [d-i], [d-ɨl] and [d-e] by incorrectly choosing the unmarked form, rather than choosing the most frequent form. Young Ah Do, MIT

  20. Faithfulness to a Base • Claim: Child avoidance and selection • In order to satisfy highly ranked OO-F constraints (McCarthy 1999). • Assumption: unmarked form as a base of Korean noun paradigm (Albright 2008). • BD-Ident constraints, a kind of OO-F, start high in children’s grammar. Young Ah Do, MIT

  21. BD-Ident and Child outputs • BD-Ident[obs,cor]/_[+high,-back, +syl] >> *[obs,cor][+high,-back, +syl] Young Ah Do, MIT

  22. Toward Adults’ Stage • Claim • Child outputs are due to wrongly promoted BD-Ident constraints. • Adults’ grammar • BD-Idents constraints are demoted to their target low ranking (i.e., Markedness >> BD-Ident) • Intermediate stage • Attempt on the most frequent variant. • (maybe add something here)? Young Ah Do, MIT

  23. Question • Question • Assuming a set of BD-Ident, Markedness, Ident-IO constraints, and assuming an input corpus with alternations of varying frequencies, do we predict a three-stage learning process? • Answer: Yes! • (Assuming learning alternations as ranking constraints in their target positions, how can a learner re-rank constraints correctly?) Young Ah Do, MIT

  24. The Goal of Learning Simulation • To see if children can learn alternations purely by the distribution of alternations. • Specifically, • The more a given alternation occurs in the data, the more the relevant BD-Ident constraints will be demoted. Young Ah Do, MIT

  25. The Learner • Given • Grammar consisting of a set of constraints with initial weight • Initial weight: BD-Ident >> M >> IO-F (McCarthy 1999, ? for M >> IO ) • Constraints based on feature specification BD-Ident (Base as unmarked form :Albright 2008) Markedness IO-F (Input as underlying form: Ko 1989) Young Ah Do, MIT

  26. Learning as Weighting Constraints • Given • Outputs with frequency • Frequency of variants according to Corpus count (Jun 2009) • Learning • The learner encounters surface forms violating constraints • Violation of constraints are accessed by Perl script. Young Ah Do, MIT

  27. Weighting Constraints • Learning • Weight constraints according to the frequency of violations in the data. • Weighting constraints • Using MaxEnt (Maximum Entropy: Goldwater & Johnson 2003) • MaxEnt Grammar Tool (Hayes 2009) • After learning • A set of trained (MaxEnt) weights for a grammar • the predicted probabilities assigned to each candidate Young Ah Do, MIT

  28. Weighting Constraints using MaxEnt Young Ah Do, MIT

  29. Learning Data • Korean nouns • Obstruent-final (labial, coronal & velar) • Lateral-final • Nasal-final • Inflection • Nominative : -i • Accusative : -ɨl • Locative/goal : -e • Locative/source : -esə Young Ah Do, MIT

  30. Learning Phonotactics • Phonotactics • Intersonorant flapping: tal taɾ-i ‘moon-nom’ • Intersonorant voicing : pap  pab-i ‘rice-nom’ mak  mag-i ‘scene-nom’ • Palatalization : tikɨt tikɨci ‘alphabet [t]-nom’ kɨth  kɨch-i ‘end-nom’ mas  maʃ-i ‘taste-nom’ Young Ah Do, MIT

  31. Learning Alternations • Alternations of obstruent-finals • Violation of BD-Ident: [t]  [s, ch, th, c, t] • Violation of IO-F: /s, ch, th, c, t/  [s, ch, th, c, t] • Alternations of non-obstruent-finals • Violation of BD-Ident: [p,k]  [ph, kh, k’] • Violation of IO-F: /ph, kh, k’/  [p, k] Young Ah Do, MIT

  32. Three Age Groups • Simulation of different age groups • The older, the more inputs they get. • An example of accusatives • Ratio adapted from corpus count s 52%, ch21 %, th 18%, c 2 %, t 0% 5.2, 2.1, 1.8, 0.2, 0 .. 52, 21, 18, 2, 0 …… 5200, 2100, 1800, 200, 0 Young Ah Do, MIT

  33. The 1st Stage • BD-Ident >>M >> IO-F Interaction of BD-Ident and Markedness constraints. • IO-F constraints are too low to influence the outcome. • URs (Input) are assumed to be discovered later (ref ?). Young Ah Do, MIT

  34. The 1st Stage • Mastery of some phonotactics • Intersonorant voicing *[+son][-v][+son] >> *[+son, +v] >> BD-Id [v] • Intersonorant flapping *[+son][+lat][+son] >> *[ɾ] >> BD-Id [lat] • Partial mastery of palatalization *[+son][+ant, -dis][+son] >> *[ʃ] >> BD-Id [+ant,-dis] *[th][+high, -back, +syl] >> BD-Id [th]/_ [+high, -back, +syl] Young Ah Do, MIT

  35. Faithful Form to a Base • No mastery of palatalization [ti]  [ci] BD-Id [obs,cor]/_ [+high, -back, +syl] >> *[obs,cor][+high, -back, +syl] • The most probable outputs that the grammar predicts • Nom: d-i (illegal) • Acc: d-ɨl (unattested) • Loc: d-e(unattested) • Source: d-esə (unattested) Young Ah Do, MIT

  36. Success of Predicting Child Forms • Predicted forms are what young children attempt in early stage. • [pat] unmarked • [pad-i] using base form • Finding • Young children’s outputs are predicted by feeding the learner a small number of alternation data of Korean. Young Ah Do, MIT

  37. The 2nd Stage Feeding more data …… Young Ah Do, MIT

  38. The 2nd Stage • IO-F constraints are still inactive. • Mastery of palatalization BD-Id [obs, cor]/_ [+high, -back, +syl] >> *[obs, cor][+high, -back, +syl] • Mastery of some alternations Young Ah Do, MIT

  39. The 2nd Stage • The most probable outputs are the most frequent adult variants, in general. CorpusSimulation s-i s-i s-ɨl s-ɨl s-e, th-e th-e th-esə s-esə Young Ah Do, MIT

  40. The Final Stage Feeding more data …… Young Ah Do, MIT

  41. The Final Stage • M >> BD-Ident • Some M constraints remain higher than IO-F constraints and some are demoted. • Variation predicted! Young Ah Do, MIT

  42. Predictability of Variants • /path-e/ Observed Predicted • [path-e] 74350.5614804325079656 • [pas-e] 2165 0.3885079149244645 • [pach-e] 120 0.056734328452862 [pad-e] 0 1.0198046038491932E-9 • [pac-e] 20 3.0068321707463542E-4 Young Ah Do, MIT

  43. Predicted Preference • Pattern match with corpus • Overall • [s] >> [ch], [th] >> [c] , [t] • Before –e, -esə • [th-e] >> [ch-e], [th-esə] >> [ch-esə] • Before -ɨl • [ch-ɨl] >> [th-ɨl] Young Ah Do, MIT

  44. Conclusions • Child form due to highly ranked OO-F constraints. • Children can demote constraints just by exposing the rankings to probabilistic data from an adult speech corpus. • That is, without intrinsic bias, the statistics of Korean give rise to the attested learning stages. Young Ah Do, MIT

  45. Simpler Modeling? • Simpler alternative without any reference to OT or OO-F constraints? • Children use the frequencies of alternant to determine which phonological rules to acquire first (suggested by anonymous WCCFL reviewer). Young Ah Do, MIT

  46. Simpler Modeling ? • Pre-[-ɨl] constraints (Jun 2007) • s/_ɨl >> ch/_ɨl >> th/_ɨl >> c/_ɨl, t/_ɨl • p/_ɨl >> ph/_ɨl • k/_ɨl >> kh/_ɨl Earlier Later Young Ah Do, MIT

  47. Evidence for arguing against this proposal?? Young Ah Do, MIT

  48. Reference Young Ah Do, MIT

  49. Thanks Young Ah Do, MIT

More Related