1 / 38

Towards Understanding Brain Connectivity: Methods and Interactions

Explore functional vs. effective connectivity, interactions in brain networks, and models in structural equation modeling. Analyze psychophysiological interactions and factorial designs in connectivity research.

nickolasc
Télécharger la présentation

Towards Understanding Brain Connectivity: Methods and Interactions

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Methods for Dummies 2008-9 Introduction to connectivity PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELLING Karine Gazarian and Carmen Tur London, February 11th, 2009

  2. Index 0. Preface I. Origins of connectivity II. Different approaches of connectivity a. Functional connectivity b. Effective connectivity III. Interactions • Factorial design • Psychophysiological interactions IV. Structural Equation Modelling V. Conclusions

  3. Preface ? Physicist Level of the Talk Clinician Wish of doing a talk about connectivity

  4. I. Origins of connectivity “Connectionism” Functional localization Gall – 19th century A certain function was localised in a certain anatomic region in the cortex Goltz – 19th century Critizied Gall’s theory of functional localization Evidence provided by dysconnection syndromes Functional segregation A certain function was carried out by certain areas/cells in the cortex but they could be anatomically separated Functional specialization Functional integration Networks: Interactions among specialised areas Specialised areas exist in the cortex

  5. II. Different approaches to connectivity Functional segregation Functional integration Networks Connectivity Functional connectivity Effective connectivity No model-based Simple correlations between areas Its study allows us to speak about temporal correlations among activation of different anatomic areas These correlations do not reflect teleologically meaningful interactions Model-based It allows us to speak about the influence that one neuronal system exerts over another It attempts to disambiguate correlations of a spurious sort from those mediated by direct or indirect neuronal interactions

  6. II. Different approaches of connectivity – Functionalconnectivity stimulus Region i Region k Time βik~ Functional connectivity What? Relationship between the activity of 2 different areas How?Principle Component Analysis (PCA), which is done by Singular Value Decomposition (SVD)  eigenvariates and eigenvalues obtained Why? To summarise patterns of correlations among brain systems  Find those spatio-temporal patterns of activity which explain most of the variance in a series of repeated measurements.

  7. II. Different approaches of connectivity – Effective connectivity stimulus A known pathway is tested Region i Region k Time STATIC MODELS DYNAMIC MODEL • xkβik ~ Effective connectivity • What? Real amount of contribution of one area (contribution of the activity of one area) to another. • How? It takes into account functionalconnectivity (correlations between areas), the whole activation in one region and interactions between different factors • Types of analysis to assess effective connectivity: • PPI – psychophysiological interactions • SEM – structural equation modeling • DCM – dynamic causal model

  8. III. Interactions a. FACTORIAL DESIGN • Study design where two or more factors are involved within a task • Aim: to look at the interaction between these factors  to look at the effect that one factor has on the responses due to another

  9. III. Interactions a. FACTORIAL DESIGN An example: Investigation of interaction between motor activation and time (Friston et al. 1992)

  10. III. Interactions a. FACTORIAL DESIGN Cognitive task BOLD signal Distracting task During the memory task PP V5 V5 V2 PFC Psychological context Attention – No attention TYPES OF INTERACTIONS PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGICAL PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL

  11. III. Interactions a. FACTORIAL DESIGN An example: Dual-task interference paradigms (Fletcher et al. 1995)

  12. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS V5 V2 Psychological context Attention – No attention Buchel and Friston Cerebral cortex 1997 • Studies where we try to explain the physiological responsein one part of the brain in terms of an interaction between prevalence of a sensorimotor or cognitive process and activity in another part of the brain • An example: interaction between activity in region V1 and some psychological parameter (e.g. attention vs no attention) in explaining the variation in activity in region V5

  13. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS ? Attention Activation in region i (e.g. V5 activity) No attention Activation in region k (e.g. V2 activity) Here the interaction can be seen as a significant difference in the regression slopes of V5 activity on V2 activity when assessed under two attentional conditions

  14. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS Two possible perspectives on this interaction… • We could have that V5 activity/response reflects: • A change of the contribution from V2 by attention • A modulation of attention-specific responses by V2 inputs

  15. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS V5 V2 Psychological context Attention – No attention Mathematical explanation y = b1*(x1 X x2)+b2*x1 + b3*x2 + e H0: b1is = 0 H1: b1is ≠ 0 and p value is < 0.05 Interaction term Physiological activity in V5 Interaction between activity in V2 and psychological context We want to test H0

  16. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS HRF basic function Neurobiological process: Where these interactions occur? Hemodynamic vs neural level Hemodynamic responses – BOLD signal – reflect the underlying neural activity ? But interactions occur at a NEURAL LEVEL And we know: (HRFxV2) X (HRFxAtt) ≠ HRFx(V2XAtt) ≠ Gitelman et al. Neuroimage 2003

  17. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS BOLD signal in V2 HRF basic function Neural activity in V2 Psychological variable x Gitelman et al. Neuroimage 2003 Neurobiological process: Where these interactions occur? Hemodynamic vs neural level SOLUTION: 1- Deconvolve BOLD signal corresponding to region of interest (e.g. V2) 2- Calculate interaction term considering neural activity psychological condition x neural activity 3- Re-convolve the interaction term using HRF

  18. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS V2 Attention – No attention Att No Att How can we do this in SPM? Practical example from SPM central page We want to assess whether the influence that V2 exerts over other areas from visual cortex (V5) depends on the status of a certain psychological condition (presence vs. absence of attention) V5 http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/data/attention/

  19. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS How can we do this in SPM? I. GLM analysis 1. Estimate GLM Y= X. β+ ε

  20. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS How can we do this in SPM? I. GLM analysis 2. Extract time series Meaning?To summarise the evolution in time and space of the activation of a certain region Place?At region of interest (e.g. V2)  region used as explanatory variable Procedure? Principle Component Analysis (done by Singular Value Decomposition)  To find those spatio-temporal patterns of activity which explain most of the variance of our dataset (i.e. activity in V1 over time)  these patterns are represented by the eigenvectors  the variance of these eigenvectors is represented by eigenvalues Reason? To include (the most important) eigenvalues in the model  we transform dynamic information (movement in time & space) into STATIC information (saved as a new matrix)  we will work with this static information  PPI is a STATIC MODEL

  21. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS y y y z z z x x x How can we do this in SPM? I. GLM analysis 2. Extract time series Y= X.β+ ε+ C.V2.β V2 activity y x z Time We add information about spatio-temporal patterns of activity which best explains our data into the previous model

  22. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS Y= β.X+ ε+β.C.V2 β(Att-NoAtt) + βiXi ~ βc.V2 Electrical activity HRF basic function BOLD signal How can we do this in SPM? II. PPI analysis • 1. Select (from the previous equation-matrix) those parameters we are interested in, i.e. • - Psychological condition: Attention vs. No attention • - Activity in V2 • 2. Deconvolve physiological regressor (V2) transform BOLD signal into electrical activity

  23. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS Electrical activity HRF basic function BOLD signal How can we do this in SPM? II. PPI analysis 3. Calculate the interaction term V2x(Att-NoAtt) 4. Convolve the interaction term V2x(Att-NoAtt) 5. Put into the model this convolved term: y = β1[V2x(Att-NoAtt)] + β2V2 + β3(Att-No-Att) + βiXi + e H0: β1 = 0 6. Create a t-contrast [1 0 0 0] to test H0 at 0.01 of significance

  24. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS Fixation (V1) Psychological context Attention – No attention How can we do this in SPM? II. PPI analysis 7. Obtain image V2 In this example For Dummies y = β1[V2x(Att-NoAtt)] + β2V2 + β3(Att-No-Att) [+ βiXi + e]

  25. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS How can we do this in SPM? II. PPI analysis 7. Obtain image BOLD activity (whole brain) H1: β1is ≠ 0 and p value is < 0.05 Interaction between activity in V2 and psychological condition (attention vs. no attention) y = β1[V2x(Att-NoAtt)] + β2V2 + β3(Att-No-Att) [+ βiXi + e]

  26. III. Interactions – b. PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS The end (of PPI…)

  27. SEMStructural Equation Modelling Karine Gazarian

  28. Definition • Structural Equation Moldelling (SEM) or otherwise called ‘path analysis’ is a multivariate tool that is used to test hypotheses regarding the influences among interacting variables. • Unlike PPI, combines an anatomical model and the inter-regional covariances of activity. • Uses estimation of parameters that define the strength of connections between brain areas in question (path coefficients), rather than activity in individual variables.

  29. A bit of history • Since 1920s and in economics, psychology and social sciences. • In functional imaging since early 1990s: • Animal autoradiographic data • Human PET data (McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1991) • fMRI (Büchel and Friston, 1997)

  30. y 1 y 2 y y 1 2 y 3 y 3 To start with…

  31. y 1 y 2 y 3 Innovations - independent residuals, driving the region stochastically To start with… y1 = z1 y2 = b12y1 + b32y3 + z2 b12 b13 b32 y3 = b13y1 + z3 y2 = f (y1 y3) + z

  32. includes only paths of interest

  33. Estimate path coefficients (b12,13,32 ) using a standard estimation algorithm • assumed some value of the innovations • implied covariance

  34. y 1 y 2 y 3 Alternative models

  35. Limitations • Static model • Inference about the parameters is obtained by iteratively constraining the model (As opposed to DCM - good example of a dynamic causal model, which allows to infer the connectivity parameters in one step) • The causality is inferred at the hemodynamic level, rather than at the more realistic neuronal level (DCM)

  36. Conclusions • Functional segregation vs. functional integration • Functional connectivity vs. effective connectivity • Three main types of analysis to study effective connectivity • PPI  STATIC MODEL • SEM STATIC MODEL • DCM DYNAMIC MODEL

  37. FURTHER READING… http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/mfd/page2/page2.html http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/SPM http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/data/attention/ Friston KJ, Frith CD, Passingham RE, et al (1992). Motor practice and neuropsychological adaptation in the cerebellum: a positron tomography study. Proc R Soc Lond B (1992) 248, 223-228. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle, PF & Frackowiak, RSJ. Functional Connectivity: The principle-component analysis of large data sets, J Cereb Blood Flow & Metab (1993) 13, 5-14 Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Grasby PM et al. Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. Brain (1995) 118, 401-416 Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR et al. Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging. Neuroimage (1997) 6, 218-229 Buchel C & Friston KJ. Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: Cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling & fMRI. Cerebral Cortex (1997) 7, 768-778 Buchel C & Friston KJ. Assessing interactions among neuronal systems using functional neuroimaging. Neural Networks (2000) 13; 871-882. Ashburner J, Friston KJ, Penny W. Human Brain Function 2nd EDITION (2003) Chap 18-20 Gitelman DR, Penny WD, Ashburner J et al. Modeling regional and neuropsychologic interactions in fMRI: The importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. Neuroimage (2003) 19; 200-207. Slides from previous years

  38. Introduction to connectivity SPECIAL THANKS TO ANDRE MARREIROS Thanks for your attention London, February 11th, 2009

More Related